scholarly journals The Role of Intuition and Formal Thinking in Kant, Riemann, Husserl, Poincare, Weyl, and in Current Mathematics and Physics

2019 ◽  
Vol 22 (1) ◽  
pp. 1-53
Author(s):  
Luciano Boi

Abstract According to Kant, the axioms of intuition, i.e. space and time, must provide an organization of the sensory experience. However, this first orderliness of empirical sensations seems to depend on a kind of faculty pertaining to subjectivity, rather than to the encounter of these same intuitions with the real properties of phenomena. Starting from an analysis of some very significant developments in mathematical and theoretical physics in the last decades, in which intuition played an important role, we argue that nevertheless intuition comes into play in a fundamentally different way to that which Kant had foreseen: in the form of a formal or “categorical” yet not sensible intuition. We show further that the statement that our space is mathematically three-dimensional and locally Euclidean by no means follows from a supposed a priori nature of the sensible or subjective space as Kant claimed. In fact, the three-dimensional space can bear many different geometrical and topological structures, as particularly the mathematical results of Milnor, Smale, Thurston and Donaldson demonstrated. On the other hand, it has been stressed that even the phenomenological or perceptual space, and especially the visual system, carries a very rich geometrical organization whose structure is essentially non-Euclidean. Finally, we argue that in order to grasp the meaning of abstract geometric objects, as n-dimensional spaces, connections on a manifold, fiber spaces, module spaces, knotted spaces and so forth, where sensible intuition is essentially lacking and where therefore another type of mathematical idealization intervenes, we need to develop a new form of intuition.

Author(s):  
Evgeniy Konopatskiy ◽  
Andrey Bezditnyi

The paper describes an approach to solid modeling of geometric objects in the form of an organized three-parameter set of points in three-dimensional space. The relevance of the research topic is due to the widespread use of solid-state models in various branches of science and technology, mechanical engineering, construction and medicine. Solid-state computer models are currently one of the basic computer graphics tools and an integral part of computer- aided design and calculation systems. It is widely used as one of the control elements of CNC machines and 3D printing, the development of information systems in the design and construction of buildings and structures, finite element calculations of deformed states in aircraft and mechanical engineering, their manufacture in medicine, etc. The choice of point calculus as a mathematical apparatus for the analytical description of solid models of geometric objects is substantiated. Examples of modeling sets of elliptical bodies and toroidal bodies in a simplex of three-dimensional space are given.


2000 ◽  
Vol 44 (21) ◽  
pp. 3-394-3-397 ◽  
Author(s):  
Harvey S. Smallman ◽  
Elaine Schiller ◽  
Craig A. Mitchell

3-D displays populated with realistic 3-D icons have been touted as making good “at a glance” displays. Do they promote more rapid Situation Awareness (SA) than comparable 2-D displays? If so, is it the display format (2-D vs. 3-D) or the nature of the symbols (realistic icons vs. non-realistic symbols) populating the displays that matters, or both? Three groups of 13 participants observed a 9 minute naval air defense scenario. The first group saw it depicted in 3-D with icons, the second group saw it depicted in 2-D with icons and the third group saw it in 2-D with symbols. In each condition, the scenario was stopped every 30 seconds and we assessed ability to recall the attributes of four random tracks with an online questionnaire. We measured Endlesy's (1995) level 1 SA: the perception of elements of the display. SA for the 3-D display increased fastest over the course of the scenario. However, it started from one third the level of that for the 2-D symbol display and it took 4 minutes to reach 2-D levels. The advantages the 3-D display did confer were for those attributes that were visually explicit in the 3-D icons but available only in pop-up text boxes in the 2-D conditions. Similarly, depicting heading explicitly with the 2-D icons was superior to that with the 2-D symbols. The benefits of 3-D displays may sometimes stem from indirect application of good design principles, such as making certain information visually explicit, rather than from depicting three-dimensional space, per se. It remains an open question whether 2-D displays can be designed with comparable explicit analog coding.


2021 ◽  
Vol 8 ◽  
Author(s):  
Changkai Bu ◽  
Lan Jin

Glycosaminoglycans (GAGs) constitute a considerable fraction of the glycoconjugates found on cellular membranes and in the extracellular matrix of virtually all mammalian tissues. The essential role of GAG-protein interactions in the regulation of physiological processes has been recognized for decades. However, the underlying molecular basis of these interactions has only emerged since 1990s. The binding specificity of GAGs is encoded in their primary structures, but ultimately depends on how their functional groups are presented to a protein in the three-dimensional space. This review focuses on the application of NMR spectroscopy on the characterization of the GAG-protein interactions. Examples of interpretation of the complex mechanism and characterization of structural motifs involved in the GAG-protein interactions are given. Selected families of GAG-binding proteins investigated using NMR are also described.


2012 ◽  
Vol 588-589 ◽  
pp. 2051-2054
Author(s):  
Ben Tu Li ◽  
Zhi Chao Yuan

In three-dimensional space, by using the method of a priori estimation, we have studied the complex-valued GL equation, which has the 2 -th power of the nonlinear term. We have proved that the existence of the global attractor of this equation with the problem of period and border value, and we have studied the dimensionality of Hausdorff and fractal of the global attractor.


2015 ◽  
Vol 25 (05) ◽  
pp. 839-873 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kewei Zhang ◽  
Antonio Orlando ◽  
Elaine Crooks

We apply compensated convex transforms to define a multiscale Hausdorff stable method to extract intersections between smooth compact manifolds represented by their characteristic functions or as point clouds embedded in ℝn. We prove extraction results on intersections of smooth compact manifolds and for points of high curvature. As a result of the Hausdorff–Lipschitz continuity of our transforms, we show that our method is stable against dense sampling of smooth manifolds with noise. Examples of explicitly calculated prototype models for some simple cases are presented, which are also used in the proofs of our main results. Numerical experiments in two- and three-dimensional space, and applications to geometric objects are also shown.


Leonardo ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 53 (1) ◽  
pp. 63-69
Author(s):  
Jasmina Stevanov ◽  
Johannes M. Zanker

The dogmatic nature of Piet Mondrian’s neoplasticism manifesto initiated a discourse about translating aesthetic ideals from paintings to 3D structures. Mondrian rarely ventured into architectural design, and his unique interior design of “Salon de Madame B … à Dresden” was not executed. The authors discuss physical constraints and perceptual factors that conflict with neoplastic ideals. Using physical and virtual models of the salon, the authors demonstrate challenges with perspective projections and show how such distortions could be minimized in a cylinder. The paradoxical percept elicited by a “reverspective” Mondrian-like space further highlights the essential role of perceptual processes in reaching neoplastic standards of beauty.


Perception ◽  
1996 ◽  
Vol 25 (7) ◽  
pp. 783-795 ◽  
Author(s):  
Thomas V Papathomas ◽  
Akos Feher ◽  
Bela Julesz ◽  
Yehoshua Zeevi

A study of size interactions of objects in three-dimensional space is reported. The canonical form of the Ebbinghaus illusion—test circles surrounded by large or small inducers—was used. Both monocularly visible (M) and purely cyclopean (C) objects were displayed stereoscopically to isolate the monocular and cyclopean components of the illusion. The results of two experiments indicate that: (i) depth plays a significant role when the test circles are cyclopean, but not when they are monocularly visible; and (ii) the size of C objects is affected equally by C and M inducers, but the size of M objects is affected much more strongly by M than by C inducers. In conclusion, possible explanations are offered for the main trends in the data, the most interesting of which is that cyclopean tests seem to be interacting only with the cyclopean component of monocularly visible inducers.


2013 ◽  
Vol 36 (5) ◽  
pp. 571-587 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kathryn J. Jeffery ◽  
Aleksandar Jovalekic ◽  
Madeleine Verriotis ◽  
Robin Hayman

AbstractWe have argued that the neurocognitive representation of large-scale, navigable three-dimensional space is anisotropic, having different properties in vertical versus horizontal dimensions. Three broad categories organize the experimental and theoretical issues raised by the commentators: (1) frames of reference, (2) comparative cognition, and (3) the role of experience. These categories contain the core of a research program to show how three-dimensional space is represented and used by humans and other animals.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document