scholarly journals Order effects in transitive inference: does the presentation order of social information affect transitive inference in social animals?

Author(s):  
Takashi Hotta ◽  
Lyndon A. Jordan ◽  
Tomohiro Takeyama ◽  
Masanori Kohda
Perception ◽  
1978 ◽  
Vol 7 (1) ◽  
pp. 91-96 ◽  
Author(s):  
Donald G Jamieson ◽  
William M Petrusic

The accuracy of many perceptual comparisons depends greatly on the order in which the to-be-compared stimuli are presented. With comparisons of durations around 300 ms, these presentation-order effects do not diminish, even with extended practice, when feedback about response accuracy is withheld. Providing such feedback greatly diminishes presentation-order effects and coincidentally produces substantial increases in response accuracy. The feedback acts in part through inducing response biases and in part through changes in sensitivity. The contradiction between studies which report time-order errors in duration comparison and those which do not is attributable to differences in the use of information feedback.


1971 ◽  
Vol 32 (2) ◽  
pp. 587-592
Author(s):  
W. F. Landers ◽  
D. C. Cogan ◽  
R. R. Hart

90 preschool and school age children were required to judge the orientation of simple triangular forms of varying completeness presented in one of three orders: increasing completeness, decreasing completeness, and random. Results indicated that number of correct responses varied as a function of age, degree of closure, and presentation order. A reliable interaction between age and presentation order indicated that the random presentation order was more difficult for the younger children. These results were taken as substantial support for the perceptual development viewpoint typified by Gibson, Hebb, and Piaget.


2012 ◽  
Vol 74 (7) ◽  
pp. 1499-1511 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mats P. Englund ◽  
Åke Hellström

Perception ◽  
1996 ◽  
Vol 25 (1_suppl) ◽  
pp. 42-42
Author(s):  
W H Ehrenstein ◽  
A N Sokolov

Identical stimuli receive higher category ratings when presented in a positively skewed distribution (smaller stimuli occur more often than larger) and lower ratings in a negatively-skewed distribution (larger stimuli occur more often). This frequency effect has been explained by a tendency to assign the same categories to the same stimuli (‘consistency model’ by Haubensak, 1992 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance18 303) rather than by a tendency to assign the same number of stimuli to each category. The consistency model postulates the prime importance of stimuli that occur early in a sequence, with the underlying assumption that more frequent stimuli have higher probability of occurring earlier. Thus, presentation order rather than frequency of stimuli might be decisive. We asked whether a ‘frequency’ effect would still obtain when stimuli are in fact presented with equal frequency, but in a sequence derived from positively or negatively skewed source samples. Ratings of visual velocity were obtained for a dot that started to move at 0.5 deg below fixation in leftward or rightward direction for 2, 2.5, or 3 deg. Five velocities (3.0, 4.5, 6.0, 7.5, 9.0 deg s−1) were presented 10 times each (in a balanced combination of stimulus duration and displacement) and were rated by 32 subjects using three categories (slow, medium, fast). Our results clearly support Haubensak's model: identical velocities were rated higher/lower in presentation sequences that mimicked the order effects of positive/negative skewing, respectively. Moreover, computer simulation of the Haubensak model reveals good agreement between predicted and observed results.


Author(s):  
Daniel Oro

In social animals, perturbations may trigger specific behavioural responses with consequences for dispersal and complex population dynamics. Perturbations raise the need for information gathering in order to reduce uncertainty and increase resilience. Updated information is then shared within the group and social behaviours emerge as a self-organized process. This social information factoralizes with the size of the group, and it is finally used for making crucial decisions about, for instance, when to leave the patch and where to go. Indeed, evolution has favoured philopatry over dispersal, and this trade-off is challenged by perturbations. When perturbations accumulate over time, they may decrease the suitability of the patch and erode the philopatric state until crossing a tipping point, beyond which most individuals decide to disperse to better areas. Initially, the decision to disperse is led by a few individuals, and this decision is copied by the rest of the group in an autocatalytic way. This feedback process of social copying is termed runaway dispersal. Furthermore, social copying enhances the evolution of cultural and technological innovation, which may cause additional nonlinearities for population dynamics. Social information gathering and social copying have also occurred in human evolution, especially after perturbations such as climate extremes and warfare. In summary, social feedback processes cause nonlinear population dynamics including hysteresis and critical transitions (from philopatry to patch collapses and invasions), which emerge from the collective behaviour of large ensembles of individuals.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document