chromatic adaptation
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

433
(FIVE YEARS 42)

H-INDEX

46
(FIVE YEARS 3)

Author(s):  
Yiqian Li ◽  
Siyuan Chen ◽  
Minchen Wei ◽  
Xiandou Zhang
Keyword(s):  

2021 ◽  
Vol 2021 (29) ◽  
pp. 368-373
Author(s):  
Yuechen Zhu ◽  
Ming Ronnier Luo

The goal of this study was to investigate the chromatic adaptation under extreme chromatic lighting conditions using the magnitude estimation method. The locations of the lightings on CIE1976 u′v′ plane were close to the spectrum locus, so the colour purity was far beyond the previous studies, and the data could test the limitations of the existing models. Two psychophysical experiments were carried out, and 1,470 estimations of corresponding colours were accumulated. The results showed that CAT16 gave a good prediction performance for all the chromatic lightings except for blue lighting, and the degree of adaptation was relatively high, that is, D was close to 1. The prediction for blue lightings was modified, the results showed the performance of CAM16 could be improved by correcting the matrix instead of the D values.


2021 ◽  
Vol 2021 (29) ◽  
pp. 166-169
Author(s):  
Hui Fan ◽  
Ming Ronnier Luo ◽  
Yuechen Zhu

The goal of this study was to investigate the time course characteristics of chromatic adaptation under highly saturated illuminants. A psychophysical experiment with neutral matching method was conducted on a mobile display at different luminance levels. Models of chromatic adaptation degree against duration of time were fitted using a proportional rate growth function. The upper limit and growth rate of adaptation degree were studied. It was found that higher adapting luminance and lower display luminance led to higher degree and faster speed of chromatic adaptation. This study also proposed the time to achieve stable chromatic adaptation.


2021 ◽  
Vol 15 ◽  
Author(s):  
O. Scott Gwinn ◽  
Talia L. Retter ◽  
Sean F. O’Neil ◽  
Michael A. Webster

Exposure to a face can produce biases in the perception of subsequent faces. Typically, these face aftereffects are studied by adapting to an individual face or category (e.g., faces of a given gender) and can result in renormalization of perceptions such that the adapting face appears more neutral. These shifts are analogous to chromatic adaptation, where a renormalization for the average adapting color occurs. However, in color vision, adaptation can also adjust to the variance or range of colors in the distribution. We examined whether this variance or contrast adaptation also occurs for faces, using an objective EEG measure to assess response changes following adaptation. An average female face was contracted or expanded along the horizontal or vertical axis to form four images. Observers viewed a 20 s sequence of the four images presented in a fixed order at a rate of 6 Hz, while responses to the faces were recorded with EEG. A 6 Hz signal was observed over right occipito-temporal channels, indicating symmetric responses to the four images. This test sequence was repeated after 20 s adaptation to alternations between two of the faces (e.g., horizontal contracted and expanded). This adaptation resulted in an additional signal at 3 Hz, consistent with asymmetric responses to adapted and non-adapted test faces. Adapting pairs have the same mean (undistorted) as the test sequence and thus should not bias responses driven only by the mean. Instead, the results are consistent with selective adaptation to the distortion axis. A 3 Hz signal was also observed after adapting to face pairs selected to induce a mean bias (e.g., expanded vertical and expanded horizontal), and this signal was not significantly different from that observed following adaption to a single image that did not form part of the test sequence (e.g., a single image expanded both vertically and horizontally). In a further experiment, we found that this variance adaptation can also be observed behaviorally. Our results suggest that adaptation calibrates face perception not only for the average characteristics of the faces we experience but also for the gamut of faces to which we are exposed.


2021 ◽  
pp. 147715352110343
Author(s):  
D Durmus

Correlated color temperature (CCT) is a one-dimensional metric that aims to quantify the perceived visual quality of nominal white light sources. It is often used as a proxy for the color quality of light sources due to its ease of use. However, CCT lacks the accuracy in communicating color information for research purposes. Two light sources with identical CCTs can appear perceptually different, and these differences are not estimated by CCT due to the loss of information caused by reducing spectral power distribution of light sources into a one-dimensional metric. Using supplemental metrics in addition to CCT, providing the absolute spectral power distribution of light sources in graphical and tabular form, and documenting and accounting for potential confounding factors, such as chromatic adaptation, can increase the validity of research results, improve the repeatability of studies, and help address replication concerns.


2020 ◽  
Vol 34 (4) ◽  
pp. 39-46
Author(s):  
SeonYoung Yoon ◽  
YoungShin Kwak ◽  
YoungJun Seo ◽  
HyoSu Kim

2020 ◽  
Vol 2020 (28) ◽  
pp. 252-257
Author(s):  
Mark D. Fairchild

Recent data has shown that the process of chromatic adaptation might be asymmetrical, or irreversible, and that this effect might be more than simply a manifestation of the time course of adaptation. This paper introduces a simple modification of the von Kries chromatic adaptation transform, referred to as vK20, that can account for the asymmetry in chromatic adaptation through inclusion of previous adapting conditions. Also introduced is a new reference chromaticity (∼15000K) for degree of adaptation that seems more physiologically plausible than the commonly used equal-energy (EE) illuminant or CIE illuminant D65.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document