Directional tuning curves, elementary movement detectors, and the estimation of the direction of visual movement

1990 ◽  
Vol 30 (4) ◽  
pp. 603-614 ◽  
Author(s):  
J.H. Van Hateren
1996 ◽  
Vol 75 (3) ◽  
pp. 1163-1176 ◽  
Author(s):  
P. Hammond ◽  
J. N. Kim

1. Single binocularly driven complex neurons in cat striate cortex were recorded extracellularly under nitrous oxide-oxygen-halothane anesthesia and muscle relaxant. Orientational/directional tuning was initially derived for each eye in turn, with sine wave gratings of optimal spatial frequency and velocity, while the other eye viewed a uniform field. 2. For the dominant eye, previously concealed suppression was revealed against elevated levels of firing induced with a conditioning grating, drifting continuously in the preferred direction, simultaneously presented to the nondominant eye. During steady-state binocular conditioning, orientational/directional tuning was reestablished for the dominant eye. In a subset of cells, tuning curves during conditioning were also derived for the reverse configuration, i.e., nondominant eye tuning, dominant eye conditioning: results were qualitatively identical to those for conditioning through the nondominant eye. 3. Neurons were initially segregated into five groups, according to the observed suppression profiles induced at nonoptimal orientations/directions during conditioning: Type 1, suppression centered on orthogonal directions; Type 2, suppression around null directions; Type 3, null suppression combined with orthogonal suppression; Type 4, lateral suppression, maximal for directions immediately flanking those inducing excitation; and Type 5, the residue of cells, totally lacking suppression or showing complex or variable suppression. 4. Sharpness of (excitatory) tuning was correlated with directionality and with class of suppression revealed during binocular conditioning. Direction-biased neurons were more sharply orientation tuned than direction-selective neurons; similarly, neurons exhibiting lateral or orthogonal suppression during conditioning were more sharply tuned than neurons with null suppression. 5. Application of suboptimal directions of conditioning weakened the induced suppression but altered none of its main characteristics. 6. The relationship between excitation, suppression, and spatial frequency was investigated by comparing tuning curves for the dominant eye at several spatial frequencies, without and during conditioning. End-stopped neurons preferred lower spatial frequencies and higher velocities of motion than non-end-stopped neurons. Confirming previous reports, suppression in some neurons was still present for spatial frequencies above the cutoff frequency for excitation, demonstrating the tendency for suppression to be more broadly spatial frequency tuned than excitation. 7. Scatterplots of strength of suppression, in directions orthogonal and opposite maximal excitation, partially segregated neurons of Types 1-3. Clearer segregation of Types 1-4 was obtained by curve-fitting to profiles of suppression, and correlating half-width of tuning for suppression with the angle between the directions of optimal suppression and optimal excitation in each neuron. 8. Two interpretations are advanced-the first, based on three discrete classes of inhibition, orthogonal, null and lateral; the second, based on only two classes, orthogonal and null/lateral--in which null and lateral suppression are manifestations of the same inhibitory mechanism operating, respectively, on broadly tuned direction-selective or on sharply tuned direction-biased neurons. Orthogonal suppression may be untuned for direction, whereas lateral and null suppression are broadly direction tuned. Within each class, suppression is more broadly spatial frequency tuned than excitation. 9. It is concluded that orientational/directional selectivity of complex cells at different spatial frequencies is determined by the balance between tuned excitation and varying combinations of relatively broadly distributed or untuned inhibition.


1993 ◽  
Vol 70 (6) ◽  
pp. 2632-2646 ◽  
Author(s):  
D. R. Wylie ◽  
T. Kripalani ◽  
B. J. Frost

1. Extracellular recordings were made from 235 neurons in the vestibulocerebellum (VbC), including the flocculus (lateral VbC), nodulus (folium X), and ventral uvula (ventral folium IXc,d), of the anesthetized pigeon, in response to an optokinetic stimulus. 2. The optokinetic stimuli consisted of two black and white random-dot patterns that were back-projected onto two large tangent screens. The screens were oriented parallel to each other and placed on either side of the bird's head. The resultant stimulus covered the central 100 degrees x 100 degrees of each hemifield. The directional tuning characteristics of each unit were assessed by moving the largefield stimulus in 12 different directions, 30 degrees apart. The directional tuning curves were performed monocularly or binocularly. The binocular directional tuning curves were performed with the direction of motion the same in both eyes (in-phase; e.g., ipsi = upward, contra = upward) or with the direction of motion opposite in either eye (antiphase; e.g., ipsi = upward, contra = downward). 3. Mossy fiber units (n = 17) found throughout folia IXa,b and IXc,d had monocular receptive fields and exhibited direction selectivity in response to stimulation of either the ipsilateral (n = 12) or contralateral (n = 5) eye. None had binocular receptive fields. 4. The complex spike (CS) activity of 218 Purkinje cells in folia IXc,d and X exhibited direction selectivity in response to the large-field visual stimulus moving in one or both visual fields. Ninety-one percent of the cells had binocular receptive fields that could be classified into four groups: descent neurons (n = 112) preferred upward motion in both eyes; ascent neurons (n = 14) preferred downward motion in both eyes; roll neurons (n = 33) preferred upward and downward motion in the ipsilateral and contralateral eyes, respectively; and yaw neurons (n = 40) preferred forward and backward motion in the ipsilateral and contralateral eyes, respectively. Within all groups, most neurons (70%) showed an ipsilateral dominance. 5. For most binocular neurons (91%), the maximum depth of modulation occurred with simultaneous stimulation of both eyes, compared with monocular stimulation of the dominant eye alone. For the translation neurons (descent and ascent), binocular inphase stimulation produced the maximum depth of modulation, whereas for the rotation neurons (roll and yaw), binocular antiphase stimulation produced the maximum depth of modulation. 6. There was a clear functional segregation of the translation and rotation neurons.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)


2012 ◽  
Vol 107 (11) ◽  
pp. 3144-3154 ◽  
Author(s):  
Andrew G. Richardson ◽  
Tommaso Borghi ◽  
Emilio Bizzi

Neurons in the primary motor cortex (M1) have been shown to have persistent, memory-like activity following adaptation to altered movement dynamics. However, the techniques used to study these memory traces limited recordings to only single sessions lasting no more than a few hours. Here, chronically implanted microelectrode arrays were used to study the long-term neuronal responses to repeated experience with perturbing, velocity-dependent force fields. Force-field–related neuronal activity within each session was similar to that found previously. That is, the directional tuning curves of the M1 neurons shifted in a manner appropriate to compensate for the forces. Next, the across-session behavior was examined. Long-term learning was evident in the performance improvements across multiple force-field sessions. Correlated with this change, the neuronal population had smaller within-session spike rate changes as experience with the force field increased. The smaller within-session changes were a result of persistent across-session shifts in directional tuning. The results extend the observation of memory traces of newly learned dynamics and provide further evidence for the role of M1 in early motor memory formation.


1990 ◽  
Vol 63 (6) ◽  
pp. 1529-1543 ◽  
Author(s):  
M. S. Gizzi ◽  
E. Katz ◽  
R. A. Schumer ◽  
J. A. Movshon

1. We consider the consequences of the orientation selectivity shown by most cortical neurons for the nature of the signals they can convey about the direction of stimulus movement. On theoretical grounds we distinguish component direction selectivity, in which cells are selective for the direction of movement of oriented components of a complex stimulus, from pattern direction selectivity, or selectivity for the overall direction of movement of a pattern irrespective of the directions of its components. We employed a novel test using grating and plaid targets to distinguish these forms of direction selectivity. 2. We studied the responses of 280 cells from the striate cortex and 107 cells from the lateral suprasylvian cortex (LS) to single sinusoidal gratings to determine their orientation preference and directional selectivity. We tested 73 of these with sinusoidal plaids, composed of two sinusoidal gratings at different orientations, to study the organization of the directional mechanisms within the receptive field. 3. When tested with single gratings, the directional tuning of 277 oriented cells in area 17 had a mean half width of 20.6 degrees, a mode near 13 degrees, and a range of 3.8-58 degrees. Simple cells were slightly more narrowly tuned than complex cells. The selectivity of LS neurons for the direction of moving gratings is not markedly different from that of neurons in area 17. The mean direction half width was 20.7 degrees. 4. We evaluated the directional selectivity of these neurons by comparing responses to stimuli moved in the optimal direction with those elicited by a stimulus moving in the opposite direction. In area 17 about two-thirds of the neurons responded less than half as well to the non-preferred direction as to the preferred direction; two-fifths of the units responded less than one-fifth as well. Complex cells showed a somewhat greater tendency to directional bias than simple cells. LS neurons tended to have stronger directional asymmetries in their response to moving gratings: 83% of LS neurons showed a significant directional asymmetry. 5. Neurons in both areas responded independently to each component of the plaid. Thus cells giving single-lobed directional-tuning curves to gratings showed bilobed plaid tuning curves, with each lobe corresponding to movement in an effective direction by one of the two component gratings within the plaid. The two best directions for the plaids were those at which one or other single grating would have produced an optimal response when presented alone.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)


1997 ◽  
Vol 78 (2) ◽  
pp. 1120-1134 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christian Quaia ◽  
Lance M. Optican

Quaia, Christian and Lance M. Optican. Model with distributed vectorial premotor bursters accounts for the component stretching of oblique saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 78: 1120–1134, 1997. During oblique saccades, the durations of the horizontal and vertical components are stretched until they are approximately equal. Models of the saccadic system have been proposed that provide a mechanism for that stretching. However, they fail to simulate the pattern of activity recorded from premotor medium lead burst neurons (MLBNs) in the brain stem. A new model of the saccadic system is proposed that accounts for both the component stretching of oblique movements and the pattern of activity recorded in MLBNs. MLBNs that project to horizontal (or vertical) motoneurons actually have a wide span of on-directions (the direction associated with the largest discharge) around the cardinal direction. We infer from the wide span of their on-directions that, at the level of individual MLBNs, the vectorial signal present in spatially organized structures (e.g., the superior colliculus) is not decomposed into the separate horizontal and vertical components represented by the motoneurons. Nonetheless, all prior models of the saccadic system have decomposed the vectorial premotor command into horizontal and vertical commands at the level of the MLBNs. That decomposition was explicit, because individual MLBNs, with a sine- or cosine-shaped directional tuning curve, were used. We propose here that the decomposition into horizontal and vertical commands is carried out only at the level of the motoneurons. This decomposition is implicit, because no single MLBN encodes the horizontal or vertical command; the command only exists implicitly in the activity of the population of MLBNs. The new vectorial burster model correctly simulates the pattern of activity recorded in primate MLBNs, and the components of its oblique saccades are stretched. Two mechanisms contribute to this stretching: the distribution of MLBN tuning curves and the inhibition exerted by the contralateral population of MLBNs. In contrast, feedback control of the saccade contributes negligibly to the stretching. Even though the vectorial burster model predicts a component stretching, it is not constrained to produce perfectly straight oblique saccades because no trajectory control is implemented. The amount of curvature depends on the similarity of the horizontal and vertical systems (both neural and mechanical). In this model, stretching is interpreted simply as a side effect of the properties of the MLBNs' tuning curves. The distributed MLBNs of the vectorial burster model forces the general organization of the saccadic system to be reconsidered. We propose that a distributed architecture in which several different neural systems cooperate is needed.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ningyu Zhang ◽  
Roddy M Grieves ◽  
Kate J Jeffery

A class of neurons showing bidirectional tuning in a two-compartment environment was recently discovered in dysgranular retrosplenial cortex (dRSC). We investigated here whether these neurons possess a more general environmental symmetry-encoding property, potentially useful in representing complex spatial structure. We report that directional tuning of dRSC neurons reflected environment symmetry in onefold, twofold and fourfold-symmetric environments: this was the case not just globally, but also locally within each sub-compartment. Thus, these cells use environmental cues to organize multiple directional tuning curves, which perhaps sometimes combine via interaction with classic head direction cells. A consequence is that both local and global environmental symmetry are simultaneously encoded even within local sub-compartments, which may be important for cognitive mapping of the space beyond immediate perceptual reach.


1981 ◽  
Vol 69 (5) ◽  
pp. 1374-1385 ◽  
Author(s):  
Robert V. Harrison ◽  
Jean–Marie Aran ◽  
Jean–Paul Erre
Keyword(s):  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document