scholarly journals Crowding and Binding: Not All Feature Dimensions Behave in the Same Way

2019 ◽  
Vol 30 (10) ◽  
pp. 1533-1546
Author(s):  
Amit Yashar ◽  
Xiuyun Wu ◽  
Jiageng Chen ◽  
Marisa Carrasco

Humans often fail to identify a target because of nearby flankers. The nature and stages at which this crowding occurs are unclear, and whether crowding operates via a common mechanism across visual dimensions is unknown. Using a dual-estimation report ( N = 42), we quantitatively assessed the processing of features alone and in conjunction with another feature both within and between dimensions. Under crowding, observers misreported colors and orientations (i.e., reported a flanker value instead of the target’s value) but averaged the target’s and flankers’ spatial frequencies (SFs). Interestingly, whereas orientation and color errors were independent, orientation and SF errors were interdependent. These qualitative differences of errors across dimensions revealed a tight link between crowding and feature binding, which is contingent on the type of feature dimension. These results and a computational model suggest that crowding and misbinding are due to pooling across a joint coding of orientations and SFs but not of colors.

2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
Amit Yashar ◽  
Xiuyun Wu ◽  
Jiageng Chen ◽  
Marisa Carrasco

Humans often fail to identify a target because of nearby flankers. The nature and stage(s) at which this 'crowding' occurs are unclear, and whether crowding operates via a common mechanism across visual dimensions is unknown. Using a dual estimation report, we quantitatively assessed the processing of each feature alone and in conjunction with another feature both within and between dimensions. Crowding emerged due to confusion between orientations or colors of target and flankers, but averaging of their spatial frequencies (SFs). Furthermore, crowding of orientation and color were independent, but crowding of orientation and SF were interdependent. This qualitative difference of crowding errors across dimensions revealed a tight link between crowding and 'illusory conjunctions' (mis-binding of feature dimensions). These results and a computational model suggest that crowding and illusory conjunction in the visual periphery are due to pooling across a joint coding of orientation and spatial frequencies but not of color.


1990 ◽  
Vol 5 (6) ◽  
pp. 547-555 ◽  
Author(s):  
D. I. Flitcroft

AbstractAccommodation is more accurate with polychromatic stimuli than with narrowband or monochromatic stimuli. The aim of this paper is to develop a computational model for how the visual system uses the extra information in polychromatic stimuli to increase the accuracy of accommodation responses. The proposed model is developed within the context of both trichromacy and also the organization of spatial and chromatic processing within the visual cortex.The refractive error present in the retinal image can be estimated by comparing image quality with and without small additional changes in refractive state. In polychromatic light, the chromatic aberration of the eye results in differences in ocular refractive power for light of different wavelengths. As a result, the refractive state of the eye can be estimated by comparing image quality in the three types of cone photoreceptor. The ability of cortical neurons to perform such comparisons on image quality with a crude form of spatial-frequency analysis is examined theoretically. It is found that spatially band-pass chromatically opponent neurons (that may correspond to double opponent neurons) can perform such calculations and that chromatic cues to accommodation are extracted most effectively by neurons responding to spatial frequencies of between 2 and 8 cycles/deg.


2010 ◽  
Vol 21 (3) ◽  
pp. 452-460
Author(s):  
Xi LIU ◽  
Zhong-Zhi SHI ◽  
Zhi-Wei SHI ◽  
Zhi-Ping SHI

2011 ◽  
Vol 21 (6) ◽  
pp. 1297-1305 ◽  
Author(s):  
Xishun Wang ◽  
Xi Liu ◽  
Zhongzhi Shi ◽  
Hongjian Sui

2008 ◽  
Vol 51 (5) ◽  
pp. 470-478 ◽  
Author(s):  
ZhiWei Shi ◽  
ZhongZhi Shi ◽  
Xi Liu ◽  
ZhiPing Shi

Author(s):  
J.M. Cowley

The problem of "understandinq" electron microscope imaqes becomes more acute as the resolution is improved. The naive interpretation of an imaqe as representinq the projection of an atom density becomes less and less appropriate. We are increasinqly forced to face the complexities of coherent imaqinq of what are essentially phase objects. Most electron microscopists are now aware that, for very thin weakly scatterinq objects such as thin unstained bioloqical specimens, hiqh resolution imaqes are best obtained near the optimum defocus, as prescribed by Scherzer, where the phase contrast imaqe qives a qood representation of the projected potential, apart from a lack of information on the lower spatial frequencies. But phase contrast imaqinq is never simple except in idealized limitinq cases.


Author(s):  
Henry I. Smith ◽  
D.C. Flanders

Scanning electron beam lithography has been used for a number of years to write submicrometer linewidth patterns in radiation sensitive films (resist films) on substrates. On semi-infinite substrates, electron backscattering severely limits the exposure latitude and control of cross-sectional profile for patterns having fundamental spatial frequencies below about 4000 Å(l),Recently, STEM'S have been used to write patterns with linewidths below 100 Å. To avoid the detrimental effects of electron backscattering however, the substrates had to be carbon foils about 100 Å thick (2,3). X-ray lithography using the very soft radiation in the range 10 - 50 Å avoids the problem of backscattering and thus permits one to replicate on semi-infinite substrates patterns with linewidths of the order of 1000 Å and less, and in addition provides means for controlling cross-sectional profiles. X-radiation in the range 4-10 Å on the other hand is appropriate for replicating patterns in the linewidth range above about 3000 Å, and thus is most appropriate for microelectronic applications (4 - 6).


Author(s):  
K.-H. Herrmann ◽  
E. Reuber ◽  
P. Schiske

Aposteriori deblurring of high resolution electron micrographs of weak phase objects can be performed by holographic filters [1,2] which are arranged in the Fourier domain of a light-optical reconstruction set-up. According to the diffraction efficiency and the lateral position of the grating structure, the filters permit adjustment of the amplitudes and phases of the spatial frequencies in the image which is obtained in the first diffraction order.In the case of bright field imaging with axial illumination, the Contrast Transfer Functions (CTF) are oscillating, but real. For different imageforming conditions and several signal-to-noise ratios an extensive set of Wiener-filters should be available. A simple method of producing such filters by only photographic and mechanical means will be described here.A transparent master grating with 6.25 lines/mm and 160 mm diameter was produced by a high precision computer plotter. It is photographed through a rotating mask, plotted by a standard plotter.


Author(s):  
Joachim Frank

Compared with images of negatively stained single particle specimens, those obtained by cryo-electron microscopy have the following new features: (a) higher “signal” variability due to a higher variability of particle orientation; (b) reduced signal/noise ratio (S/N); (c) virtual absence of low-spatial-frequency information related to elastic scattering, due to the properties of the phase contrast transfer function (PCTF); and (d) reduced resolution due to the efforts of the microscopist to boost the PCTF at low spatial frequencies, in his attempt to obtain recognizable particle images.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document