scholarly journals On Accepting the Null Hypothesis of Conditional Independence in Partial Correlation Networks: A Bayesian Analysis

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Donald Ray Williams ◽  
Giovanni Briganti ◽  
Paul Linkowski ◽  
Joris Mulder

Partial correlation networks have emerged as an increasingly popular model for studyingmental disorders. Although conditional independence is a fundamental concept in networkanalysis, which corresponds to the null hypothesis, the focus is typically to detect and thenvisualize non-zero partial correlations (i.e., the “edges” connecting nodes) in a graph. As aresult, it may be tempting to interpret a missing edge as providing evidence for itsabsence—analogously to misinterpreting a non-significant p-value. In this work, we firstestablish that a missing edge is incorrectly interpreted as providing evidence for conditionalindependence, with examples spanning from substantive applications to tutorials thatinstruct researchers to misinterpret their networks. We then go beyond misguided“inferences” and establish that null associations are interesting in their own right. In thefollowing section, three illustrative examples are provided that employ Bayesian hypothesistesting to formally evaluate the null hypothesis, including a reanalysis of twopsychopathology networks, confirmatory testing to determine whether a particularpost-traumatic stress disorder symptom is disconnected from the network, and attenuationdue to correcting for covariates. Our results shed light upon conditionally independentsymptoms and demonstrate that a missing edge does not necessarily correspond toevidence for the null hypothesis. These findings are accompanied with a simulation studythat provides insights into the sample size needed to accurately detect null relations. Weconclude with implications for both clinical to theoretical inquiries.

1996 ◽  
Vol 21 (3) ◽  
pp. 264-282 ◽  
Author(s):  
András Vargha ◽  
Tamás Rudas ◽  
Harold D. Delaney ◽  
Scott E. Maxwell

It was recently demonstrated that performing median splits on both of two predictor variables could sometimes result in spurious statistical significance instead of lower power. Not only is the conventional wisdom that dichotomization always lowers power incorrect, but the current article further demonstrates that inflation of apparent effects can also occur in certain cases where only one of two predictor variables is dichotomized. In addition, we show that previously published formulas claiming that correlations are necessarily reduced by bivariate dichotomization are incorrect. While the magnitude of the difference between the correct and incorrect formulas is not great for small or moderate correlations, it is important to correct the misunderstanding of partial correlations that led to the error in the previous derivations. This is done by considering the relationship between partial correlation and conditional independence in the context of dichotomized predictor variables.


2021 ◽  
Vol 11 (2) ◽  
pp. 31-50
Author(s):  
S.L. Artemenkov

Network modeling, which has emerged in recent years, can be successfully applied to the consideration of relationships between measurable psychological variables. In this context, psychological variables are understood as directly affecting each other, and not as a consequence of a latent construct. The article describes regularization methods that can be used to effectively assess the sparse and interpretable network structure based on partial correlations of psychological indicators. An overview of the glasso regularization procedure using EBIC model selection for evaluating an ordered sparse network of partial correlations is presented. The issues of performing this analysis in R in the presence of normal and non-normal data distribution are considered, taking into account the influence of the hyperparameter, which is manually set by the researcher. The considered approach is also interesting as a way to visualize possible causal connections between variables. This review bridges the gap related to the lack of an accessible description in Russian of this approach, which is still uncommon in Russia and at the same time promising.


2018 ◽  
Vol 26 (5) ◽  
pp. 524-530 ◽  
Author(s):  
Wole Akosile ◽  
David Colquhoun ◽  
Ross Young ◽  
Bruce Lawford ◽  
Joanne Voisey

Objectives: There are some psychosocial factors that have similar importance to biological factors in the genesis of coronary diseases. However, reasons for high rates of coronary heart disease in individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are yet to be fully elucidated. Using a meta-analysis, we investigated the longitudinal relationship between PTSD and coronary heart disease (CHD) as an independent factor in the aetiology of CHD. Methods: The databases of Medline, EBSCOhost and Psychoinfo were electronically searched for relevant articles. Results: The pooled hazard ratio (HR) for the magnitude of the relationship between PTSD and CHD was an HR of 1.61, and p-value of p < 0.0005, 95% confidence interval (CI) [1.46–1.77] before adjustment for depression in nine studies ( N = 151,144) that met inclusion criteria. The HR estimates for the seven depression-adjusted estimates was 1.46, and a p-value of p < 0.0005, 95% CI[0.26–1.69]. Conclusions: This study demonstrates an association between CHD and PTSD.


2013 ◽  
Vol 36 (6) ◽  
pp. 614-614 ◽  
Author(s):  
Martin Desseilles ◽  
Catherine Duclos

AbstractDuring dreaming, as well as during wakefulness, elaborative encoding, indexing and ancient art of memory (AAOM) techniques, such as the method of loci, may coincide with emotion regulation. These techniques shed light on the link between dreaming and emotional catharsis, post-traumatic stress disorder, supermemorization during sleep as opposed to wakefulness, and the developmental role of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep in children.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document