scholarly journals DBS Artifact Removal Through Template Subtraction

2020 ◽  
Vol 21 (2) ◽  
Author(s):  
Marco Emilio Vazquez ◽  
Enrico Opri ◽  
Brandon Parks ◽  
Gunduz Aysegul

Deep Brain Stimulation (DBS) typically results in the formation of large signal artifacts in electrophysiological recordings in the surrounding regions of the stimulated area. This can prove to be problematic, as it makes the study of physiological responses in Local Field Potentials (LFPs), and consequently Event Related Potentials (ERPs) quite challenging. Research has been done in attempts to attenuate the effects of these large artifacts through various ways, most commonly through blind suppression, function fitting, template subtraction, and adaptive filters. However, many of these methods have proven to only be useful within the context of surface recordings (EEGs) and not for LFPs. In our research, we utilize template subtraction and extend it to the context of LFPs, in an attempt to uncover more effectively the underlying physiological responses to DBS.

1990 ◽  
Vol 64 (3) ◽  
pp. 903-914 ◽  
Author(s):  
T. H. Bullock ◽  
M. H. Hofmann ◽  
F. K. Nahm ◽  
J. G. New ◽  
J. C. Prechtl

1. Compound field potentials were recorded with up to 18 microelectrodes in comb, brush, or spear arrays on and in the optic tectum and with suction electrodes from the distal stump of the cut optic nerve and from the optic nerve head in the opened eye in elasmobranchs and teleosts. Diffuse light flashes of different durations and submaximal intensities were delivered in trains with regular or irregular interstimulus intervals (ISI). 2. Event-related potentials (ERPs) are visible in single trials and begin at 50-200 ms after an "oddball" flash, especially one that is slightly weaker, briefer, or delayed by as little as 6% of ISI, compared with the more frequent stimulus. ERPs to the opposite condition are not of the same form or size. 3. One or more stimuli were omitted from a train or the train terminated after various conditioning times. Deflections occur beyond the expected visual-evoked potentials (VEPs) to the last flash and are called omitted-stimulus potentials (OSPs). They occur on schedule--approximately 100 ms after the next flash would be due--almost independent of intensity, duration, or conditioning time. They are considered to be ERPs without any necessary implication or denial of a temporally specific expectation. 4. Three components of OSP occur alone or in combination: an initial fast peak, a slow wave, and an oscillatory spindle up to ls or more in duration. This resembles the OFF response to steady light. 5. All these components are already present in the retina with optic nerve cut. 6. The same mean ISI with a high proportion of jitter gives OSPs with only slightly longer latencies and smaller amplitudes; the OSP acts as though the retina makes an integrated prediction of ISI, intensity, and duration. 7. During a conditioning train the equilibrium between excitation and inhibition after each flash changes according to frequency, intensity, duration, and conditioning time; the VEP reflects this in a shape unique to the ISI; inhibition increases rapidly after each flash and then decays slowly according to the recent mean ISI. This allows rebound disinhibition after missing, weak, or delayed flashes (OSP or ERP) or causes an altered VEP after a longer or stronger oddball. 8. It seems unlikely that the OSP or oddball ERP in fish tectum is equivalent to mammalian ERPs under the same regime or signals higher cognitive events, because they are already present in the retina, require flash frequencies greater than 1 Hz, and grow with frequency up to and beyond flicker fusion.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mary McIntosh ◽  
Benjamin Corrigan ◽  
Roberto Gulli ◽  
Guillaume Doucet ◽  
Julio Martinez-Trujillo ◽  
...  

Abstract The hippocampi and mesial temporal lobes play a central role in episodic memory and associative learning. It is unclear how unexpected experience influences learning. Hippocampal sharp wave ripples (SWR) are an electrical biomarker of memory consolidation. We tracked when and where SWR occur during 2 tasks. Local field potentials were recorded in the hippocampi, entorhinal cortices and amygdalae of non-human primates (NHP; n=3) performing reversal and associative learning tasks in a 3D virtual environment. Our results show hippocampal SWR occurred when learned task contingencies were unexpectedly altered. Surprise rewards and reward denial were associated with SWR rates 9.8x and 8.0x greater than expected rewards. The highest density of SWR occurred in zones where errors were made. SWR were preceded by event-related potentials in the amygdala but not entorhinal cortex. Our results suggest that SWR generation in primates may prioritize behaviourally relevant experience for commitment to memory to allow flexible learning.


2021 ◽  
pp. 030573562097869
Author(s):  
Alice Mado Proverbio ◽  
Francesca Russo

We investigated through electrophysiological recordings how music-induced emotions are recognized and combined with the emotional content of written sentences. Twenty-four sad, joyful, and frightening musical tracks were presented to 16 participants reading 270 short sentences conveying a sad, joyful, or frightening emotional meaning. Audiovisual stimuli could be emotionally congruent or incongruent with each other; participants were asked to pay attention and respond to filler sentences containing cities’ names, while ignoring the rest. The amplitude values of event-related potentials (ERPs) were subjected to repeated measures ANOVAs. Distinct electrophysiological markers were identified for the processing of stimuli inducing fear (N450, either linguistic or musical), for language-induced sadness (P300) and for joyful music (positive P2 and LP potentials). The music/language emotional discordance elicited a large N400 mismatch response ( p = .032). Its stronger intracranial source was the right superior temporal gyrus (STG) devoted to multisensory integration of emotions. The results suggest that music can communicate emotional meaning as distinctively as language.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jiayi Bao ◽  
Xinbo Song ◽  
Yan Li ◽  
Yinjie Bai ◽  
Qianxiang Zhou

Abstract The mental workload of subjects was tested in different lighting conditions, with color temperature ranging from 3000K to 6500K and illuminance 300 lx to 1000 lx. We used both psychological and physiological responses for evaluation. The former was based on the NASA-TLX scores, and the latter was based on EEG P3b analysis of event-related potentials with the “oddball” paradigm experimental task. Results show that the P3b amplitudes are significantly affected by color temperature (P = 0.009) and illuminance (P = 0.038) levels. Office environment with 3000K color temperature and 750 lx illumination, which exerts the lowest mental workload, is the most suitable for working. However, the interaction between color temperature and illuminance in affecting the mental workload of participants is not clear. This work provides more appropriate lighting choices with color temperature and illuminance in order to reduce people’s mental workload in office setting.


2009 ◽  
Vol 102 (4) ◽  
pp. 2375-2386 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jeremiah Y. Cohen ◽  
Richard P. Heitz ◽  
Jeffrey D. Schall ◽  
Geoffrey F. Woodman

Despite nearly a century of electrophysiological studies recording extracranially from humans and intracranially from monkeys, the neural generators of nearly all human event-related potentials (ERPs) have not been definitively localized. We recorded an attention-related ERP component, known as the N2pc, simultaneously with intracranial spikes and local field potentials (LFPs) in macaques to test the hypothesis that an attentional-control structure, the frontal eye field (FEF), contributed to the generation of the macaque homologue of the N2pc (m-N2pc). While macaques performed a difficult visual search task, the search target was selected earliest by spikes from single FEF neurons, later by FEF LFPs, and latest by the m-N2pc. This neurochronometric comparison provides an empirical bridge connecting macaque and human experiments and a step toward localizing the neural generator of this important attention-related ERP component.


2008 ◽  
Vol 20 (10) ◽  
pp. 1903-1914 ◽  
Author(s):  
Frank Marzinzik ◽  
Michael Wahl ◽  
Gerd-Helge Schneider ◽  
Andreas Kupsch ◽  
Gabriel Curio ◽  
...  

The processing of executive control is thought to involve cortical as well as thalamic brain areas. However, the questions of how thalamic structures contribute to the control of behavior and how cortical versus thalamic processing is coordinated remain to be settled. We therefore aimed at specifying respective activations during the performance of a go/no-go task. To this end, an electroencephalogram was recorded simultaneously from scalp and thalamic electrodes in seven patients undergoing deep brain stimulation. Meanwhile, left- or right-directed precues were presented indicating with which index finger a button press should be putatively executed. Thereafter, 2 sec elapsed until a go or no-go stimulus determined if the prepared movement had to be performed or withheld. In fronto-central scalp as well as in thalamic recordings, event-related potentials upon go versus no-go instructions were expressed differentially. This task effect was unrelated to motor processes and emerged significantly prior at thalamic than at scalp level. Amplitude fluctuations of depth and scalp responses showed site- and task-dependent correlations, particularly between thalamic and no-go-related activities at frontal recording sites. We conclude that an early classification of go and no-go instructions is performed already thalamically. It further appears that this information is subsequently utilized by cortical areas engaged in the definite inhibition of the prepared action.


2008 ◽  
Vol 14 (6) ◽  
pp. 476-480 ◽  
Author(s):  
Norbert Kovacs ◽  
Istvan Balas ◽  
Lorant Kellenyi ◽  
Jozsef Janszky ◽  
Adam Feldmann ◽  
...  

2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Taha Morshedzadeh ◽  
Neil M. Drummond ◽  
Utpal Saha ◽  
Robert Chen ◽  
Milad Lankarany

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document