scholarly journals A Transferrable Change in Preferences of Floral Patterns by Bumblebees through Reward Reversal

Author(s):  
Vicki Xu ◽  
Catherine Plowright

This study examines the use behavioral transfer across perceptually similar stimuli in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens) and addresses whether foraging judgments about a floral stimulus can change in a way that contradicts direct previous experience with that stimulus. Twenty bees from each of four colonies underwent discrimination training of stimuli placed in a radial maze. Bees were trained to discriminate between two corresponding object and photograph pairs of artificial flowers, where one object and its corresponding photo were rewarding, while another object and its corresponding photo were unrewarding. Following discrimination training, one stimulus from each pair (either the object or the photo) was removed. The predictive reward values of the remaining stimuli were either switched for one group or stayed the same for another. Subsequent testing on the removed stimuli revealed foraging preferences to shift based on experience with the other stimulus in the group. For instance, bees treated a previously unrewarding object as rewarding after learning that the corresponding photograph had become rewarding. Foraging decisions depend not only on previous experience with stimuli, but also category membership.

2013 ◽  
Vol 2013 ◽  
pp. 1-8 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hamida B. Mirwan ◽  
Peter G. Kevan

Social learning occurs when one individual learns from another, mainly conspecific, often by observation, imitation, or communication. Using artificial flowers, we studied social learning by allowing test bumblebees to (a) see dead bumblebees arranged in foraging positions or (b) watch live bumblebees actually foraging or (c) communicate with nestmates within their colony without having seen foraging. Artificial flowers made from 1.5 mL microcentrifuge tubes with closed caps were inserted through the centres of blue 7 cm plastic discs as optical signals through which the bees could not forage. The reinforcer reward syrup was accessible only through holes in the sides of the tubes beneath the blue discs. Two colonies (A and B) were used in tandem along with control (C and D) colonies. No bee that was not exposed (i.e., from the control colonies (C and D)) to social learning discovered the access holes. Inside colony B, we imprisoned a group of bees that were prevented from seeing or watching. Bees that saw dead bumblebees in foraging positions, those that watched nest-mates foraging, and those that had only in-hive communication with successful foragers all foraged successfully. The means of in-hive communication are not understood and warrant intense investigation.


1986 ◽  
Vol 21 (3) ◽  
pp. 269-285 ◽  
Author(s):  
David A.L. Levy ◽  
Howard Machin

THE 1986 ELECTIONS PRODUCED ONE OF THE MOST DRAMATIC results in recent French history, after one of the most unexciting campaigns. It is clear that the marked a break with the previous experience of the Fifth Repu Is lic.What was the nature of this break? First, the campaign began in a context where defeat seemed inevitable for the President's majority in parliament. Secondly, there was a new electoral system which affected the campaign, the competition and the results. Thirdly, the results showed considerable changes in French voting behaviour. Fourthly, there were some important changes in the influence and nature of the parties themselves. Fifthly, and perhaps most importantly, despite the collapse of the two-block part system, a majority hostile to the President was elected to par l 'ament. The President responded by staying in office, and appointing the leader of the largest party in the new majority as prime minister. The other original feature of these elections was that they were held less than two years before a presidential election. Much of the changed behaviour of voters, parties and leaders reflected not only the circumstances of the elections of 1986 but also those foreseen in 1988.


Behaviour ◽  
1977 ◽  
Vol 61 (3-4) ◽  
pp. 276-302 ◽  
Author(s):  
H.I. Russock ◽  
M.W. Schein

AbstractThis study investigated the nature of social bond formation in fry of the maternal moutbrooding cichlid fish, 1'. mossambica. Attention was especially devoted to possible behavioural predispositions of the fry in addition to the effects of early social experience. Three different maternal models were used in the study: I) a black pit model, which previous research had indicated might be highly attractive to the fry; 2) a red rectangle, which previous research had indicated might be an inadequate maternal model; and 3) a preserved female T. niossambica. All fry were removed from their mother's mouth as eggs and hatched under artificial conditions so that they were never exposed to normal maternal stimuli. Separate groups of naive fry were tested for their responsiveness to all three models on days 10, 12, 16, and 22 post-hatching; under normal conditions fry are released from their mother's mouth for the first time about day 10 and leave their mother's presence about day 22 post-hatching. Other groups of fry were exposed to one of the three maternal models for varying lengths of time on day I, I through 6, 8, 10, or 12 and then tested for their responsiveness to all three models on day 10, 12, 16, or 22. It was found that all groups of naive fry of a given age exhibited the same level of responsiveness to all three maternal models. There was a peak of positive responsiveness on day 12 and a decrease by day 16; this pattern is similar to the pattern of responsiveness exhibited by maternally reared fry toward their real mother. Previous exposure to any of the models failed to prevent the decline in positive responsiveness observed in naive fry on day 16. Previous exposure to models also failed to have any effect, at any age, on the response of fry to the black pit model: fry which had been exposed to models responded at the same level as naive fry of the same age. On the other hand, previous exposure with any model often resulted in a significant decrease in positive responses to the red rectangle while such experience had an intermediate effect on later responsiveness to the preserved female T. mossambica. It was concluded that T. mossambica fry hatch with an initial perceptual schema that predisposes the fry to react to certain characteristics of the broody mother. However, when the fry are naive they initially respond to almost any object, regardless of whether or not the object fits their schema. If the object to which they initially respond does not fit their schema (i.e., the red rectangle), experience with the inadequate object will cause them to fail to respond to the inadequate object in the future; previous experience with objects that better fit their schema will also result in a similar lack of positive responsiveness. On the other hand, the fry will respond positively to a model that closely matches their schema (i.e., the black pit model) regardless of their previous experience. Finally, the fry's initial perceptual schema undergoes an irreversible developmental deterioration. This deterioration explains why the fry in the present study exhibited a significant decrease in positive responsiveness by day 16 regardless of their previous experience; it also explains why normally reared fry always leave their real mother by approximately day 22. It would be highly adaptive for the fry to ignore or avoid maternal stimuli after day 22 since an attraction to mouth-size holes could be fatal once they have left their mother.


10.28945/3914 ◽  
2017 ◽  
Vol 2 ◽  
pp. 001-025 ◽  

Dr. T. Grandon Gill, a Professor in the Information Systems and Decision Sciences Department at the University of South Florida, was traveling with his family in England when he received a strange phone message. Not being able to respond, he ignored it until—a couple of days later—he was notified that access to his personal website had been suspended (see Exhibit 1). Grandon.com had, once again, been hacked–for the 7th time. Getting his website hacked was not a new experience for Grandon Gill. In the past, however, getting the site back up and running had been a quick fix involving replacing the corrupted files. This time it was different. Based on the email and his service provider’s response, his site now contained links to PayPal phishing sites. Without significant changes, he could become complicit in fraud if the situation was not remedied. This was a problem that could no longer be ignored. After Gill had re-read the email, he pondered the various options available to him. Given the amount of trouble it was causing him, he wondered if he needed the website at all. To maintain the domain name grandon.com, which he had held for more than 20 years, all he needed to do was to put up a simple landing page with a message: “Hi, I am Grandon—go to my school account to find out more.” At the other extreme, he could completely re-engineer the site to make it much less vulnerable—a process that could take days, if not weeks. Between the two extremes, there were many other possibilities. These included changing hosts, simplifying the site so that it contained only the most critical information, dropping its WordPress component, or even going to a pure WordPress model. He had a suspicion, based on previous experience, that vulnerabilities in WordPress may have been the source of the hack. But were these vulnerabilities intrinsic to the application, or were they simply the result of his inattentive management? Whatever he decided, he needed to take action soon. It was very embarrassing, and perhaps professionally damaging, to have his site showing an unavailable message. He thought back to a popular ironic quote that said: “Good decisions come from experience, and experience comes from bad decisions.” What should he do now?


2018 ◽  
Vol 16 (1-2) ◽  
Author(s):  
Svjetlana Vranješ

Using a sample size of 200 R&D employees, this paper examines the relationship between the current salary and starting salary, previous experience, education, employees’ age and gender. The results provided by this study show that current salary is positively associated with employees’ salary at the beginning of the career and years of education. The author finds strong evidence that current salary is negatively associated with employees’ age, previous experience and gender. Furthermore, conducting cluster analysis, results provide two different groups. The first group consists of employees who are more likely to be included in the clerical type of jobs and the second group is specific to the other types of job.


1997 ◽  
Vol 129 (5) ◽  
pp. 915-925 ◽  
Author(s):  
Catherine Plowright ◽  
Nathalie Cantin-Plante

AbstractIn a laboratory experiment on the decision rule underlying the avoidance of flower revisitation by foraging bumble bees (Bombus impatiens), artificial flowers were arranged in two rows. For each flower visited, the choice was either to travel straight ahead onto a flower in the same row or to change direction and choose a flower in the other row. The distance between rows was manipulated to be smaller than, equal to, or greater than the distance between flowers in the same row. When the distances between adjacent flowers within a row and between rows were equal, a significant tendency to travel straight ahead was observed. This tendency was sensitive to changes in distance between rows, even when the distances between rows far exceeded the distance between flowers within a row. The tendency was abolished when the distance between rows was smaller than the distance between flowers within a row. A significant alternation of the bumble bee between rows (zigzag path) was observed in only one condition: when not only the distance between rows was much smaller than the distance between flowers in the same row, but also when the rows were staggered such that the bee could alternate between rows while still travelling in an almost straight line. These results show that the two variables of distance and angle are integrated, and that the choices made by bumble bees do not reflect a simple rule. Future research on tradeoffs is suggested.


1966 ◽  
Vol 19 (3) ◽  
pp. 868-870 ◽  
Author(s):  
Dorothy E. McAllister ◽  
Wallace R. McAllister ◽  
Donald K. Zellner

An initial preference for a white box with a grid floor over a gray box with a solid floor was shown by two groups of rats. For both groups, this preference increased with time. One group having 1 hr. of familiarization with a similar, white, grid box exhibited a significantly stronger preference than the other group having no previous experience with the apparatus. An interpretation based on fear of novel stimuli and the similarity of the grid box and the home cage was offered.


1963 ◽  
Vol 13 (3) ◽  
pp. 679-686 ◽  
Author(s):  
A. B. Paige ◽  
H. J. McNamara

The purposes of this experiment are further investigation of: the discrimination hypothesis vs secondary reinforcement positions with respect to resistance to extinction; the role of explicit vs non-explicit discrimination training in resistance to extinction. One group of 32 rats was given explicit discrimination training in an L-type runway involving two goal boxes of different brightness. A second group of 32 Ss had non-explicit discrimination training, i.e., an unfastened card in the goal box entrances concealed the cues until S entered the goal box. Ss were always reinforced in the goal box of one brightness and never reinforced in the goal box of the other brightness. During extinction 16 Ss in each of the above groups were extinguished with the positive cue and 16 with the negative cue. Half of these positive cue and negative cue Ss encountered a card in the goal box entrance, and the rest did not. The results showed no significant difference in alley-running or arm-running times in extinction between the positive and negative cue Ss. Using number of trials to a criterion of two successive running times, Ss extinguished with the positive cue showed greater resistance to extinction. The type of discrimination training did not influence extinction results significantly, using running time or number of trials as a criterion. The results were interpreted as generally incompatible with the discrimination hypothesis prediction and only partially in support of a secondary reinforcement position.


Behaviour ◽  
1963 ◽  
Vol 21 (1-2) ◽  
pp. 78-124 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ernst S. Reese

AbstractRearing experiments (REESE, 1962) showed that the behavior of hermit crabs toward the shells which they inhabit is fully and completely expressed the first time it is released, and therefore that the form of the behavior is not dependent upon previous experience with an adequate releasing object. The present investigation is an analysis of the sense organs and the properties of the shells, the sign stimuli, which release the fixed motor patterns comprising the shell exploration and selection behavior. Hermit crabs orient visually to objects, such as shells and pebbles, which contrast with the background and are of an appropriate size relative to the crabs.There is no behavioral evidence for visual discrimination of shape. After the initial orientation, vision plays no further role in the behavior. Likewise, olfaction is not involved in the behavior. Once the crabs have made contact with shells, all subsequent sensory information regarding the shells is supplied via mechanoreceptors, both tactile and proprioceptors of the appendages and probably of the abdomen and cephalothorax. Crabs with only a single intact appendage (cheliped or walking leg) are able to enter and ultimately select the preferred species of shell. Observations of normal behavior of crabs lacking their abdomens suggested that proprioceptors and perhaps tactile receptors of the abdomen feed-back only negative information to the higher centers controlling the behavior. Thus "no news is good news" and with ablation of the abdomen no feed-back can occur and the behavior proceeds normally. The ablation of appendages revealed compensatory adjustments by the intact appendages which take over the functional roles of the ablated appendages. By removing both the chelipeds and the first pair of walking legs, it was possible to bring two functional roles, holding the shell and exploring the aperture, into conflict on the second pair of walking legs. Although this pair of legs was used to explore the aperture, their primary function of holding the shell predominated. The properties of the shells, which are encountered sequentially by the crabs, release distinct and characteristic motor patterns which bring the crabs into the next stimulus situation, where in turn they encounter the sign stimuli which release the next fixed motor pattern. In this way the behavior proceeds step by step until the crab has entered and righted the shell. In the order in which they are encountered by the crabs, the properties of the shells which release the fixed motor patterns are: contrast with the background, movability, surface texture, external shape, aperture presence, aperture free of obstructions, internal size, shell up-side-down, internal configuration and weight. The motor patterns are reviewed in the beginning of the discussion. Although normally the fixed motor patterns follow one another in an ordered sequence, they can be released individually in isolation, that is out of context relative to the other patterns. Therefore, they are not interdependent as are the events in a reaction chain. Upon completion of the final act of the behavior, entering and righting the shell, the crabs continue to show appetitive searching behavior, characterized by a high level of motor activity and responsiveness to shells, depending upon the species and/or weight of the shells entered by the crabs. For example, if the crabs enter Tegula shells the level of the appetitive behavior is low, but if Olivella shells are entered, it remains high. The crabs are able to discriminate between the species of shells used in the investigation on the basis of either the external shape alone or the aperture alone, and although the shape of any shell, of appropriate relative size, releases the motor patterns associated with external exploratory behavior, the shape of the preferred species of shell is more stimulating to the crabs. For example, the crabs spend more time exploring the shape of Tegula shells than Olivella shells. Therefore, the sign stimuli must be thought of as having graded releasing value. An interesting recycling of the behavior was observed in conjunction with shells with their apertures partially obstructed. Cessation of the recycling appeared to be due to adaptation occurring centrally, perhaps of the releasing mechanism itself. Finally, a functional scheme is presented which explains a selection process which is not dependent upon previous experience with the stimulus objects. Two properties of the shell, weight and internal configuration, appear to contribute directly to the determination of the total stimulus value of the shell. The other properties of the shell, besides releasing specific fixed motor patterns, may also contribute to the evaluation of the shell as a stimulus object. An experiment is described to test this hypothesis. The total stimulus value of the shell determines the further responsiveness of the crabs toward shells. Thus crabs select shells by a lack or reduction of responsiveness to shells rather than by any direct comparison between shells.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document