tic suppression
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

53
(FIVE YEARS 22)

H-INDEX

15
(FIVE YEARS 2)

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Payton Beeler ◽  
Nicholas O. Jensen ◽  
Soyoung Kim ◽  
Amy Viehoever-Robichaux ◽  
Bradley L. Schlaggar ◽  
...  

Tics manifest as brief, purposeless, and involuntary movements or noises that can be suppressed temporarily with effort. In 1998, Peterson and Leckman (P&L) hypothesized that the chaotic temporal nature of tics could possess an inherent fractality, that is, have neighbor-to-neighbor correlation at all levels of time scale. However, demonstrating this phenomenon has eluded researchers for more than two decades, primarily because of the challenges associated with estimating the scale-invariant, power law exponent-called the fractal dimension Df-from a fractional Brownian noise. Here, we confirm P&L's hypothesis and establish the fractality of tics by examining year-long tic time series dataset of children diagnosed with Tourette syndrome using one-dimensional random walk models. We find that Df increases from ~1.4 to 1.75 in order of decreasing tic severity, and is correlated with the conventional YGTTS total tic score (TTS) clinical measure (p-value = 0.03). We demonstrate Df to be a sensitive parameter in examining the effect of several tic suppression conditions on the tic time series. Our findings pave the way for utilizing the fractal nature of tics as a quantitative tool for estimating tic severity and treatment effectiveness, as well as a marker for differentiating typical from functional tics.


F1000Research ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 9 ◽  
pp. 1264
Author(s):  
Jonathan K. Black ◽  
Jonathan M. Koller ◽  
Kevin J. Black

Woods and Himle developed a standardized tic suppression paradigm (TSP) for the experimental setting, to quantify the effects of intentional tic suppression in Tourette syndrome. We previously provided a computer program to facilitate recording tic occurrence and to automate reward delivery during the several experimental conditions of the TSP. The present article describes a web-based program that performs the same functions. Implementing this program on the web allows research sessions to be performed remotely, in tandem with a video calling program. Relevant data for each session, such as the timing of tics and dispensed rewards, are stored in plain text files for later analysis. Expected applications include research on Tourette syndrome and related disorders.


F1000Research ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 9 ◽  
pp. 1264
Author(s):  
Jonathan K. Black ◽  
Jonathan M. Koller ◽  
Kevin J. Black

Woods and Himle developed a standardized tic suppression paradigm (TSP) for the experimental setting, to quantify the effects of intentional tic suppression in Tourette syndrome. We previously provided a computer program to facilitate recording tic occurrence and to automate reward delivery during the several experimental conditions of the TSP. The present article describes a web-based program that performs the same functions. Implementing this program on the web allows research sessions to be performed remotely, in tandem with a video calling program. Relevant data for each session, such as the timing of tics and dispensed rewards, are stored in plain text files for later analysis. Expected applications include research on Tourette syndrome and related disorders.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Keisuke Ueda ◽  
Soyoung Kim ◽  
Deanna Greene ◽  
Kevin J. Black

Purpose of review: Tic disorders are common in the pediatric population and are differentiated from other movement disorders by tic suppressibility. Understanding the mechanism of tic suppression may provide new insights to the pathophysiology of tic disorders. This article highlights clinical phenomenology and neuronal correlates of tic suppressibility. Recent findings: Recent studies suggest that tic suppressibility exists in children shortly after onset of their tics. Moreover, those who are better able to suppress their tics have better tic outcomes. Interoceptive awareness and automatic action inhibition may be involved in tic suppression. Summary: We illustrate a possible underlying mechanism of tic suppressibility and its clinical correlations and implications. New concepts such as interoceptive awareness and action inhibition may help explain tic disorders. Further study will be useful to fill remaining knowledge gaps.


2020 ◽  
Vol 11 ◽  
Author(s):  
Natsumi Matsuda ◽  
Maiko Nonaka ◽  
Toshiaki Kono ◽  
Miyuki Fujio ◽  
Marina Nobuyoshi ◽  
...  

Brain ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 143 (3) ◽  
pp. 906-919 ◽  
Author(s):  
Vishal Rawji ◽  
Sachin Modi ◽  
Anna Latorre ◽  
Lorenzo Rocchi ◽  
Leanne Hockey ◽  
...  

Abstract The defining character of tics is that they can be transiently suppressed by volitional effort of will, and at a behavioural level this has led to the concept that tics result from a failure of inhibition. However, this logic conflates the mechanism responsible for the production of tics with that used in suppressing them. Volitional inhibition of motor output could be increased to prevent the tic from reaching the threshold for expression, although this has been extensively investigated with conflicting results. Alternatively, automatic inhibition could prevent the initial excitation of the striatal tic focus—a hypothesis we have previously introduced. To reconcile these competing hypotheses, we examined different types of motor inhibition in a group of 19 patients with primary tic disorders and 15 healthy volunteers. We probed proactive and reactive inhibition using the conditional stop-signal task, and applied transcranial magnetic stimulation to the motor cortex, to assess movement preparation and execution. We assessed automatic motor inhibition with the masked priming task. We found that volitional movement preparation, execution and inhibition (proactive and reactive) were not impaired in tic disorders. We speculate that these mechanisms are recruited during volitional tic suppression, and that they prevent expression of the tic by inhibiting the nascent excitation released by the tic generator. In contrast, automatic inhibition was abnormal/impaired in patients with tic disorders. In the masked priming task, positive and negative compatibility effects were found for healthy controls, whereas patients with tics exhibited strong positive compatibility effects, but no negative compatibility effect indicative of impaired automatic inhibition. Patients also made more errors on the masked priming task than healthy control subjects and the types of errors were consistent with impaired automatic inhibition. Errors associated with impaired automatic inhibition were positively correlated with tic severity. We conclude that voluntary movement preparation/generation and volitional inhibition are normal in tic disorders, whereas automatic inhibition is impaired—a deficit that correlated with tic severity and thus may constitute a potential mechanism by which tics are generated.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Charlotte Rae

Tourette syndrome is characterised by ‘unvoluntary’ tics, which are compulsive, yet often temporarily suppressible. The inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) is implicated in motor control, including inhibition of pre-potent actions through influences on downstream subcortical and motor regions. While tic suppression in Tourette Syndrome also engages the IFG, it is unclear whether such prefrontal control of action is also dysfunctional: Tic suppression studies do not permit comparison with control groups, and neuroimaging studies of motor inhibition can be confounded by the concurrent expression or suppression of tics. Here, patients with Tourette syndrome were directly compared to control participants when performing an intentional inhibition task during fMRI. Tic expression was recorded throughout for removal from statistical models. Participants were instructed to make a button press in response to Go cues, withheld responses to NoGo cues to, and decide whether to press or withhold to ‘Choose’ cues. Overall performance was similar between groups, for both intentional inhibition rates (% Choose-Go) and reactive NoGo inhibition commission errors. A subliminal face prime elicited no additional effects on intentional or reactive inhibition. Across participants, the task activated prefrontal and motor cortices and subcortical nuclei, including pre-supplementary motor area (preSMA), IFG, insula, caudate nucleus, thalamus, and primary motor cortex. In Tourette syndrome, activity was elevated in the IFG, insula, and basal ganglia, most notably within the right IFG during voluntary action and inhibition (Choose-Go and Choose-NoGo), and reactive inhibition (NoGo-correct). Anatomically, the locus of this IFG hyperactivation during control of voluntary action matched that previously reported for tic suppression. In Tourette syndrome, activity within the caudate nucleus was also enhanced during both intentional (Choose-NoGo) and reactive (NoGo-correct) inhibition. Strikingly, despite the absence of overt motor behaviour, primary motor cortex activity increased in patients with Tourette syndrome but decreased in controls during both reactive and intentional inhibition. Additionally, severity of premonitory sensations scaled with functional connectivity of the preSMA to the caudate nucleus, globus pallidus, and thalamus when choosing to respond (Choose-Go). Together, these results suggest that patients with Tourette syndrome use equivalent prefrontal mechanisms to suppress tics and withhold non-tic actions, but require greater IFG engagement than controls to overcome motor drive from hyperactive downstream regions, notably primary motor cortex. Moreover, premonitory sensations may cue midline motor regions to generate tics through interactions with the basal ganglia.


2020 ◽  
Vol 2 (2) ◽  
Author(s):  
Charlotte L Rae ◽  
Jim Parkinson ◽  
Sophie Betka ◽  
Cassandra D Gouldvan Praag ◽  
Samira Bouyagoub ◽  
...  

Abstract Tourette syndrome is characterized by ‘unvoluntary’ tics, which are compulsive, yet often temporarily suppressible. The inferior frontal gyrus is implicated in motor control, including inhibition of pre-potent actions through influences on downstream subcortical and motor regions. Although tic suppression in Tourette syndrome also engages the inferior frontal gyrus, it is unclear whether such prefrontal control of action is also dysfunctional: Tic suppression studies do not permit comparison with control groups, and neuroimaging studies of motor inhibition can be confounded by the concurrent expression or suppression of tics. Here, patients with Tourette syndrome were directly compared to control participants when performing an intentional inhibition task during functional MRI. Tic expression was recorded throughout for removal from statistical models. Participants were instructed to make a button press in response to Go cues, withhold responses to NoGo cues, and decide whether to press or withhold to ‘Choose’ cues. Overall performance was similar between groups, for both intentional inhibition rates (% Choose-Go) and reactive NoGo inhibition commission errors. A subliminal face prime elicited no additional effects on intentional or reactive inhibition. Across participants, the task activated prefrontal and motor cortices and subcortical nuclei, including pre-supplementary motor area, inferior frontal gyrus, insula, caudate nucleus, thalamus and primary motor cortex. In Tourette syndrome, activity was elevated in the inferior frontal gyrus, insula and basal ganglia, most notably within the right inferior frontal gyrus during voluntary action and inhibition (Choose-Go and Choose-NoGo), and reactive inhibition (NoGo-correct). Anatomically, the locus of this inferior frontal gyrus hyperactivation during control of voluntary action matched that previously reported for tic suppression. In Tourette syndrome, activity within the caudate nucleus was also enhanced during both intentional (Choose-NoGo) and reactive (NoGo-correct) inhibition. Strikingly, despite the absence of overt motor behaviour, primary motor cortex activity increased in patients with Tourette syndrome but decreased in controls during both reactive and intentional inhibition. Additionally, severity of premonitory sensations scaled with functional connectivity of the pre-supplementary motor area to the caudate nucleus, globus pallidus and thalamus when choosing to respond (Choose-Go). Together, these results suggest that patients with Tourette syndrome use equivalent prefrontal mechanisms to suppress tics and withhold non-tic actions, but require greater inferior frontal gyrus engagement than controls to overcome motor drive from hyperactive downstream regions, notably primary motor cortex. Moreover, premonitory sensations may cue midline motor regions to generate tics through interactions with the basal ganglia.


2019 ◽  
Vol 50 (12) ◽  
pp. 3944-3957 ◽  
Author(s):  
Laura Zapparoli ◽  
Antonella Macerollo ◽  
Eileen M. Joyce ◽  
Davide Martino ◽  
James M. Kilner

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document