On the relationship between the stevens exponent law and short-term storage capacity

1989 ◽  
Vol 7 (2-4) ◽  
pp. 323
Author(s):  
I. Myshkin
2009 ◽  
Vol 47 (14) ◽  
pp. 3067-3073 ◽  
Author(s):  
Frédéric Peters ◽  
Steve Majerus ◽  
Julie De Baerdemaeker ◽  
Eric Salmon ◽  
Fabienne Collette

2012 ◽  
Vol 140 (2) ◽  
pp. 158-163 ◽  
Author(s):  
Thomas Alrik Sørensen ◽  
Søren Kyllingsbæk

2013 ◽  
Vol 24 (8) ◽  
pp. 1967-1978 ◽  
Author(s):  
I. Wiegand ◽  
T. Tollner ◽  
T. Habekost ◽  
M. Dyrholm ◽  
H. J. Muller ◽  
...  

2004 ◽  
Vol 9 (1) ◽  
pp. 32-42 ◽  
Author(s):  
Thierry Olive

The dual-task paradigm recently played a major role in understanding the role of working memory in writing. By reviewing recent findings in this field of research, this article highlights how the use of the dual-task technique allowed studying the processing and short-term storage functions of working memory involved in writing. With respect to processing functions of working memory (namely, attentional and executive functions), studies investigated resource allocation, step-by-step management, and parallel coordination of the writing processes. With respect to short-term storage in working memory, experiments mainly attempted to test Kellogg's (1996) proposals on the relationship between the writing processes and the slave systems of working memory. The dual-task technique proved fruitful in understanding the relationship between writing and working memory because researchers exploited its major advantage, namely, its flexibility.


2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Zeljana Babic ◽  
Mark Schurgin ◽  
Timothy F. Brady

Working memory is a core cognitive system that actively maintains information in an accessible state to support a variety of everyday tasks. Crucially, working memory performance has frequently been shown to strongly correlate with fluid intelligence. Traditionally when these correlations have been observed, the working memory tasks involved required a high degree of manipulation and executive function, as opposed to solely utilizing short-term storage capacity. However, recent work has claimed that simple storage capacity is also correlated with fluid intelligence, and that this is driven by a particularly special and dissociable component of capacity, the ‘number of items represented’ (rather than the precision of those representations). These results have been used to argue that investigating the underlying mechanisms of capacity limitations may be critical to understanding aspects of fluid intelligence. Here we demonstrate that such correlations do not arise solely or primarily from simple storage capacity (nor a single dissociable component of capacity), but are driven by the availability of strategic encoding of different kinds of visual representations. Specifically, a working memory task that decreased the utility of storing and making use of spatial ensemble information, while holding constant the number of items to be remembered and the exact changes participants needed to detect, significantly reduced the correlation between working memory performance and fluid intelligence. Thus, despite being probed on the same items, with the same foils, at the same set size, only working memory displays that allowed for the strategic use of both item and ensemble representations correlated with fluid intelligence. These results provide evidence against the hypothesis that simple storage alone is related to fluid intelligence. They also demonstrate that participants make use of more complex and structured representations rather than solely individual item representation, and that strategic utilization of these representations is what correlates strongly with fluid intelligence.


2011 ◽  
Vol 21 (5) ◽  
pp. 569-574 ◽  
Author(s):  
Pascale Marguerite Josiane Engel de Abreu ◽  
Susan Elizabeth Gathercole ◽  
Romain Martin

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document