Supplemental Material for Something in the Way We Move: Motion Dynamics, Not Perceived Sex, Influence Head Movements in Conversation

2011 ◽  
Vol 37 (3) ◽  
pp. 874-891 ◽  
Author(s):  
Steven M. Boker ◽  
Jeffrey F. Cohn ◽  
Barry-John Theobald ◽  
Iain Matthews ◽  
Michael Mangini ◽  
...  

Gesture ◽  
2017 ◽  
Vol 16 (1) ◽  
pp. 68-99 ◽  
Author(s):  
Amal Mechraoui ◽  
Faridah Noor Binti Mohd Noor

Abstract When we speak, we do not only produce a chain of words and utterances, but we also perform various body movements that convey information. These movements are usually made with the hands and are what McNeill (1992) terms gestures. Although gesturing is universal, the way we gesture and the meanings we associate with gestures vary cross-culturally. Using a qualitative approach, this paper describes and illustrates the forms and functions of pointing gestures used by Malay speakers. The data discussed is based on 10 video recorded direction-giving interactions. Findings show that pointing among Malay speakers is achieved through the use of various manual pointing gestures and other bodily actions involving gaze, torso and head movements, which communicate distinct functions. This study has indicated that although some gesture forms and functions are shared among Malay speakers and other cultural groups, some direction-giving pointing behaviors are Malay specific.


1974 ◽  
Vol 60 (3) ◽  
pp. 735-767
Author(s):  
PETER SHEPHEARD

1. Head movement in the locust Schistocerca gregaria is mediated by 14 pairs of muscles. The normal motor output to many of these muscles has been investigated in the intact insect by recording with two intracellular microelectrodes from different fibres of a muscle during slow and fast phases of optokinetic nystagmus elicited by rotation of a striped drum in the visual field. In addition, details of the innervation pattern and muscle fibre spectrum of the muscles have been investigated by paired intracellular recording during graded stimulation of the motor nerves in the dissected preparation. Of more than 60 different axons shown histologically to innervate the neck muscles on each side, the activity of about 25 have been analysed and deductions are made about the way the muscles work together in concert to produce head movement. 2. At least four physiologically distinct types of axon innervate several muscles. These types are tonic (slow), phaso-tonic (intermediate), phasic (fast) and inhibitory. Stationary head positions are maintained by low levels of motor output in the tonic axons alone, these axons also being mainly responsible, when active at higher frequency, for producing small slow head movements. Phaso-tonic axons become progressively more active during larger or faster head movements to augment the effect of tonic axon activity. The fastest head movements are correlated especially with activity of the phasic axons. Inhibitory neurones are active during and immediately before rapid head movements, the output to individual muscles being correlated with both contraction and relaxation. 3. On the basis of their electrical responsiveness to the axons innervating them, muscle fibres have been classified into the following types: ‘phasic’, ‘intermediate’ and ‘tonic’, with the tonic class subdivided into ‘ordinary tonic’, ‘super-tonic’ and ‘super-sensitive’. Some muscles contain only a small range of fibre types, while others contain the complete range. Deductions are made about the way the different axon types interact on the different muscle fibre types to elicit various degrees of contraction or tension. 4. The slow phase motor output patterns are the result of a centrally determined programme elicited by visual input (rotation of the striped drum), and are not dependent on, but may perhaps be modified by, proprioceptive feedback. On the other hand, the fast phase of nystagmus is initiated by the central nervous system only after reference to the proprioceptive input from various sense organs of the neck and prothorax.


2018 ◽  
Vol 41 ◽  
Author(s):  
Maria Babińska ◽  
Michal Bilewicz

AbstractThe problem of extended fusion and identification can be approached from a diachronic perspective. Based on our own research, as well as findings from the fields of social, political, and clinical psychology, we argue that the way contemporary emotional events shape local fusion is similar to the way in which historical experiences shape extended fusion. We propose a reciprocal process in which historical events shape contemporary identities, whereas contemporary identities shape interpretations of past traumas.


2020 ◽  
Vol 43 ◽  
Author(s):  
Aba Szollosi ◽  
Ben R. Newell

Abstract The purpose of human cognition depends on the problem people try to solve. Defining the purpose is difficult, because people seem capable of representing problems in an infinite number of ways. The way in which the function of cognition develops needs to be central to our theories.


1976 ◽  
Vol 32 ◽  
pp. 233-254
Author(s):  
H. M. Maitzen

Ap stars are peculiar in many aspects. During this century astronomers have been trying to collect data about these and have found a confusing variety of peculiar behaviour even from star to star that Struve stated in 1942 that at least we know that these phenomena are not supernatural. A real push to start deeper theoretical work on Ap stars was given by an additional observational evidence, namely the discovery of magnetic fields on these stars by Babcock (1947). This originated the concept that magnetic fields are the cause for spectroscopic and photometric peculiarities. Great leaps for the astronomical mankind were the Oblique Rotator model by Stibbs (1950) and Deutsch (1954), which by the way provided mathematical tools for the later handling pulsar geometries, anti the discovery of phase coincidence of the extrema of magnetic field, spectrum and photometric variations (e.g. Jarzebowski, 1960).


Author(s):  
W.M. Stobbs

I do not have access to the abstracts of the first meeting of EMSA but at this, the 50th Anniversary meeting of the Electron Microscopy Society of America, I have an excuse to consider the historical origins of the approaches we take to the use of electron microscopy for the characterisation of materials. I have myself been actively involved in the use of TEM for the characterisation of heterogeneities for little more than half of that period. My own view is that it was between the 3rd International Meeting at London, and the 1956 Stockholm meeting, the first of the European series , that the foundations of the approaches we now take to the characterisation of a material using the TEM were laid down. (This was 10 years before I took dynamical theory to be etched in stone.) It was at the 1956 meeting that Menter showed lattice resolution images of sodium faujasite and Hirsch, Home and Whelan showed images of dislocations in the XlVth session on “metallography and other industrial applications”. I have always incidentally been delighted by the way the latter authors misinterpreted astonishingly clear thickness fringes in a beaten (”) foil of Al as being contrast due to “large strains”, an error which they corrected with admirable rapidity as the theory developed. At the London meeting the research described covered a broad range of approaches, including many that are only now being rediscovered as worth further effort: however such is the power of “the image” to persuade that the above two papers set trends which influence, perhaps too strongly, the approaches we take now. Menter was clear that the way the planes in his image tended to be curved was associated with the imaging conditions rather than with lattice strains, and yet it now seems to be common practice to assume that the dots in an “atomic resolution image” can faithfully represent the variations in atomic spacing at a localised defect. Even when the more reasonable approach is taken of matching the image details with a computed simulation for an assumed model, the non-uniqueness of the interpreted fit seems to be rather rarely appreciated. Hirsch et al., on the other hand, made a point of using their images to get numerical data on characteristics of the specimen they examined, such as its dislocation density, which would not be expected to be influenced by uncertainties in the contrast. Nonetheless the trends were set with microscope manufacturers producing higher and higher resolution microscopes, while the blind faith of the users in the image produced as being a near directly interpretable representation of reality seems to have increased rather than been generally questioned. But if we want to test structural models we need numbers and it is the analogue to digital conversion of the information in the image which is required.


1979 ◽  
Vol 44 (1) ◽  
pp. 3-30 ◽  
Author(s):  
Carol A. Pruning

A rationale for the application of a stage process model for the language-disordered child is presented. The major behaviors of the communicative system (pragmatic-semantic-syntactic-phonological) are summarized and organized in stages from pre-linguistic to the adult level. The article provides clinicians with guidelines, based on complexity, for the content and sequencing of communicative behaviors to be used in planning remedial programs.


ASHA Leader ◽  
2013 ◽  
Vol 18 (2) ◽  
pp. 5-7 ◽  
Author(s):  
Patty Prelock

Children with disabilities benefit most when professionals let families lead the way.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document