The automatic activation of emotion words measured using the emotional face-word Stroop task in late Chinese–English bilinguals

2017 ◽  
Vol 32 (2) ◽  
pp. 315-324 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lin Fan ◽  
Qiang Xu ◽  
Xiaoxi Wang ◽  
Fei Xu ◽  
Yaping Yang ◽  
...  
1999 ◽  
Vol 58 (4) ◽  
pp. 241-250 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kerry Kawakami ◽  
Kenneth L. Dion ◽  
John F. Dovidio

In the present study, automatic stereotype activation related to racial categories was examined utilizing a primed Stroop task. The speed of participants' ink-color naming of stereotypic and nonstereotypic target words following Black and White category primes were compared: slower naming times are presumed to reflect interference from automatic activation. The results provide support for automatic activation of implicit prejudice and stereotypes. With respect to prejudice, naming latencies tended to be slower for positive words following White than Black primes and slower for negative words following Black than White primes. With regard to stereotypes, participants demonstrated slower naming latencies for Black stereotypes, primarily those that were negatively valenced, following Black than White category primes. These findings provide further evidence of the automatic activation of stereotypes and prejudice that occurs without intention.


2010 ◽  
Vol 478 (1) ◽  
pp. 1-4 ◽  
Author(s):  
Xiang-ru Zhu ◽  
Hui-jun Zhang ◽  
Ting-ting Wu ◽  
Wen-bo Luo ◽  
Yue-jia Luo

2010 ◽  
Vol 35 (4) ◽  
pp. 285-292 ◽  
Author(s):  
Julia Avram ◽  
Felicia Rodica Balteş ◽  
Mircea Miclea ◽  
Andrei C. Miu

2008 ◽  
Vol 67 (1) ◽  
pp. 23-34 ◽  
Author(s):  
J VANHOOFF ◽  
K DIETZ ◽  
D SHARMA ◽  
H BOWMAN

PeerJ ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 9 ◽  
pp. e11267
Author(s):  
Xueling Ma ◽  
Entao Zhang

Background Major power theories assume that social power can play an important role in an individual’s goal-related behaviors. However, the specific psychological mechanisms through which this occurs remain unclear. Some studies suggested that having power enhanced individuals’ goal-related behaviors, by contrast, other studies suggested that low-power individuals were associated with a greater performance in goal-directed tasks. We were particularly interested in how social power changes individuals’ goal-related behaviors during an emotional face-word Stroop task. Method Social power was primed by asking participants to recall a past situation in which they were in a position of power (high-power individuals), or a situation in which they were lacking power (low-power individuals). Afterward, participants were asked to complete an emotional face-word Stroop task. In the task, words representing specific emotions were written in a prominent red color across a face, and these words and facial expressions were either congruent or incongruent. The participant’s task was to judge the emotion of the face while ignoring the red emotional words. Results Our behavioral data showed that these individuals displayed faster reaction time and better accuracy in congruent conditions, slower reaction time for fearful faces and worse accuracy for happy faces in both incongruent and congruent conditions. The event-related potential analyses showed that, compared with low-power individuals, high-power individuals showed greater P1 amplitudes when faced with emotional stimuli (both incongruent and congruent conditions), indicating that power affects individuals’ attention in the early sensory processing of emotional stimuli. For the N170 component, low-power individuals showed more negative amplitudes when facing emotional stimuli, indicated that low-power individuals paid more attention to the construct information of emotional stimuli. For the N450 component, compared with congruent conditions, incongruent conditions elicited more negative amplitudes for both high- and low-power individuals. More importantly, fearful faces provoked enhanced P1 amplitudes in incongruent conditions than in congruent conditions only for low-power individuals, while, happy faces elicited larger P1 amplitudes in congruent conditions than in incongruent conditions only for high-power individuals. The findings suggested that during the initial stage of stimuli processing low-power individuals are more sensitive to negative stimuli than high-power individuals. Conclusion These findings provided electrophysiological evidence that the differences in the emotional conflict process between high- and low-power individuals mainly lies in the early processing stages of emotional information. Furthermore, evidence from P1 and N170 showed that there was also a redistribution of attentional resources in low-power individuals.


2018 ◽  
Vol 15 (5) ◽  
pp. 056014 ◽  
Author(s):  
Zhenghao Guo ◽  
Xia Wu ◽  
Jianhong Liu ◽  
Li Yao ◽  
Bin Hu

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document