Self-referent Information-processing in Individuals at High and Low Cognitive Risk for Depression

1997 ◽  
Vol 11 (5-6) ◽  
pp. 539-568 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lauren B. Alloy ◽  
Lyn Y. Abramson ◽  
Laura A. Murray ◽  
Wayne G. Whitehouse ◽  
Michael E. Hogan
2003 ◽  
Vol 17 (4) ◽  
pp. 347-358 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jennifer A. Steinberg ◽  
Brandon E. Gibb ◽  
Lauren B. Alloy ◽  
Lyn Y. Abramson

Previous work has established a relationship between reports of childhood emotional maltreatment and cognitive vulnerability to depression, as well as an association between cognitive vulnerability and self-referent information-processing biases. Findings from this study of individuals at low (LR) and high (HR) cognitive risk for depression revealed a relationship between reports of childhood emotional maltreatment and current information processing biases. Specifically, individuals with greater childhood emotional maltreatment exhibited more negative self-referent information processing. Moreover, cognitive risk mediated the relationship between childhood emotional maltreatment and these information-processing biases. Testing an alternate model, information-processing biases also mediated the relationship between childhood emotional maltreatment and cognitive risk.


2002 ◽  
Vol 16 (4) ◽  
pp. 487-502 ◽  
Author(s):  
Alisa G. Crossfield ◽  
Lauren B. Alloy ◽  
Brandon E. Gibb ◽  
Lyn Y. Abramson

This study examined the role of childhood negative life events and parental inferential feedback in the development of cognitive vulnerability to depression. Students with negative cognitive styles, previously shown to be at high cognitive risk for depression, were predicted to have a greater history of negative childhood life events and negative parental inferential feedback than were students at low cognitive risk for depression. It was further predicted that parental inferential feedback would moderate the relationship between negative childhood life events and cognitive risk for depression. Finally, the associations between subsets of childhood negative life events and cognitive risk for depression were examined. No significant main effects were found for childhood negative life events or parental inferential feedback. The interaction of these 2 variables was significantly associated with cognitive risk status. Specifically, high levels of negative childhood life events in combination with negative maternal inferential feedback were associated with students’ cognitive risk for depression. When the negative childhood life events were divided into subsets, no main effects or interactions were found.


2019 ◽  
Vol 38 (1) ◽  
pp. 50-69
Author(s):  
Annaleis K. Giovanetti ◽  
JUlia C. Revord ◽  
Maria P. Sasso ◽  
Gerald J. Haeffel

Introduction: The positive effects of expressive writing on both mental and physical health are well documented. However, expressive writing may have the potential to activate negative schemas and facilitate rumination in those at high cognitive risk for depression (Yasinski, Hayes, & Laurenceau, 2016). The current research tested the hypothesis that writing using self-distancing would be more effective than traditional expressive writing in preventing depressive symptoms for those at high cognitive risk for depression. Method: Two studies using undergraduate samples (n = 104 and n = 80) were conducted to test our hypotheses. Both studies used a two-week daily writing experimental design. Results: Contrary to hypotheses, Study 1 found that individuals randomly assigned to a self-distancing writing condition reported greater levels of depressive symptoms than those assigned to a traditional expressive writing condition. The results of Study 2 replicated the results of Study 1. Participants randomly assigned to the self-distancing writing condition reported significantly greater levels of depressive symptoms than those in the expressive writing and no-writing control conditions. The effect of writing condition in Study 2 was driven by those with high levels of cognitive vulnerability. Discussion: Results suggest that writing using self-distancing should not be used for the prevention of depressive symptoms.


2006 ◽  
Vol 93 (1-3) ◽  
pp. 61-69 ◽  
Author(s):  
Brian M. Iacoviello ◽  
Lauren B. Alloy ◽  
Lyn Y. Abramson ◽  
Wayne G. Whitehouse ◽  
Michael E. Hogan

2000 ◽  
Vol 109 (3) ◽  
pp. 403-418 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lauren B. Alloy ◽  
Lyn Y. Abramson ◽  
Michael E. Hogan ◽  
Wayne G. Whitehouse ◽  
Donna T. Rose ◽  
...  

2006 ◽  
Vol 115 (1) ◽  
pp. 145-156 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lauren B. Alloy ◽  
Lyn Y. Abramson ◽  
Wayne G. Whitehouse ◽  
Michael E. Hogan ◽  
Catherine Panzarella ◽  
...  

2016 ◽  
Vol 39 ◽  
Author(s):  
Giosuè Baggio ◽  
Carmelo M. Vicario

AbstractWe agree with Christiansen & Chater (C&C) that language processing and acquisition are tightly constrained by the limits of sensory and memory systems. However, the human brain supports a range of cognitive functions that mitigate the effects of information processing bottlenecks. The language system is partly organised around these moderating factors, not just around restrictions on storage and computation.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document