scholarly journals The Good Tastes Study: Relations Between Children's Eating Behaviors and Caregivers’ Intentions to Persist in Offering Difficult-to-like Foods (OR03-02-19)

2019 ◽  
Vol 3 (Supplement_1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Susan Johnson ◽  
Kameron Moding ◽  
Abigail Flesher ◽  
Kathryn Davis ◽  
Rebecca Boenig ◽  
...  

Abstract Objectives Young children need repeated exposure to learn to like new foods but can eat only foods caregivers offer. Thus, caregiver feeding decisions are central in determining whether children receive sufficient exposure to learn to like new foods. We undertook an exploratory study to: 1) examine child behaviors that influence caregiver perceptions of their child's liking for novel foods; and to 2) identify which child behaviors are associated with caregiver intentions to continue offering new foods. Methods Infants/toddlers (n = 106; 57 males; 6 – 24 mo) were observed tasting up to 8 offers of a novel food (pureed kale) by their caregivers (94% mothers). Trained researchers coded videos for successful tastes, positive (e.g., reaching for the spoon, playing with the food) and negative (e.g., expelling the food, avoiding the spoon, crying) child eating behaviors and children's degree of acceptance of the kale (0 = refusal to 3 = anticipation). Caregiver ratings of child liking for the food (1 = Dislikes extremely to 9 = likes extremely) and their likelihood of re-offering the food (1 = not at all likely to 5 = very likely) were collected. Kruskal-Wallis tests explored differences in successful bites, frequencies of child behaviors, and caregiver ratings across child age (6 – 8.30 [mo.days]; 9 – 14.30; 15 – 24). Spearman correlations explored associations among successful tastes, child behaviors and caregiver ratings. Results Successful tastes decreased with child age (P = .000) and toddlers (15 – 24) exhibited more avoidant behavior (P = .000), cried more (P < .05), played more (P = .005), and had a lower degree of acceptance (P = .000) than infants (6 – 8.30). Perceptions of child liking and the likelihood of re-offering the kale were highly correlated (r = .63, P = .000) and did not differ by child age. Ratings of child liking and the likelihood of re-offering were: 1) negatively associated with avoidant behaviors (P < .001), crying (P < .05) or expelling kale (P < .000); and 2) positively associated with successful tastes (P < .05) and the degree of acceptance (P = .000). Conclusions Caregiver intention to continue offering a novel food was related to whether their child ate the food, how avidly the food was eaten, and how negatively their child reacted to the food. Getting children to eat and avoiding stressful feedings may be the immediate priority over building food acceptance. Funding Sources The Sugar Association.

2020 ◽  
Vol 4 (Supplement_2) ◽  
pp. 985-985
Author(s):  
Abigail Flesher ◽  
Kameron Moding ◽  
Kathryn Davis ◽  
Allyson Montalvo ◽  
Rebecca Boenig ◽  
...  

Abstract Objectives Child feeding can be challenging during the infant and toddler period when children are learning to eat complementary foods. We examined associations among children's eating behaviors, whether children were caregiver fed or self-fed, and children's intake of a novel food during two eating occasions in a laboratory setting. Methods Caregivers (n = 55, 96% mothers) were given an oatmeal/lipid nutrient supplement mixture to offer to their infants/toddlers (7–24 months; 27 boys) on 2 occasions (Baseline [V1] and after 2 weeks of home exposure [V2]). Visits were video-recorded and coded for positive (e.g., leaning in, reaching for the spoon) and negative (e.g., crying/fussing, refusals) child behaviors at V1 and V2. The number of child self-feeding attempts were coded at both visits and children were subsequently divided into 3 groups: Never (0 attempts), Sometimes (1–5 attempts), and Often self-feeds (6 + attempts). An Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was performed for each visit to determine whether amounts consumed (g) differed by self-feeding group, controlling for child age (months) and number of home exposures (at V2 only). ANCOVA was also performed to determine whether child negative behaviors differed by self-feeding group, with age and number of home exposures as covariates. Results Observed positive behaviors had no significant association with consumption. At V1, age (F = 11.5, P = .001), but not self-feeding group (F = 2.5, P = .096), was significantly related to amounts consumed. After 2 weeks of home exposure (V2), amounts consumed varied by self-feeding group (F = 3.20, P = .049) with children in the Often self-feeding group consuming significantly more (36.0 ± 5.2 g) than children in the Never (P = .045; 22.2 ± 3.8 g) and Sometimes (P = .023; 19.2 ± 5.3 g) groups. Negative behaviors also differed by self-feeding group (F = 3.46, P = .039), with children in the Often group displaying fewer negative behaviors compared to children in the Sometimes group (P = .045), controlling for age and home-exposures. Conclusions Child self-feeding resulted in greater food consumption and fewer negative child behaviors. Enabling children to learn to self-feed may be an important component of improving food acceptance as well as child and caregiver feeding experiences during the challenging toddler period. Funding Sources Global Alliance for Improved Nutrition.


2021 ◽  
Vol 5 (Supplement_2) ◽  
pp. 817-817
Author(s):  
Allison Shapiro ◽  
Abigail Flesher ◽  
Kendal Lattanzi ◽  
Renee Charlifue-Smith ◽  
Susan Johnson

Abstract Objectives Little inquiry has been undertaken regarding the interface of children's development of eating behaviors and their understanding of food-related (FR) words. Thus, we explored the relationship between young children's understanding of FR vocabulary (FR receptive language) and their acceptance of a novel food. Methods Caregivers (n = 35) and children ages 7–24 mo (n = 12 infants age &lt;12 mo; n = 23 toddlers age ≥12 mo) participated in a study in which caregivers offered a novel food (nutrition supplement added to infant oatmeal) to their children. Novel food acceptance was measured as g consumed. The Communicative Development Inventory assessed caregiver perceptions of infants’ understanding of 154 words related to food and eating (FR receptive language). A linear regression model with FR receptive language, age (toddler vs. infant) and a FR receptive language-by-age interaction was used to predict acceptance of the novel food. Covariates included infant sex and BMI z-score and a p &lt; 0.1 was chosen for this exploratory study. Results As expected, caregivers reported that infants understood fewer words as compared to toddlers (median [IQR]; 7 [13] vs. 33 [46], respectively). The relationship between FR receptive language and novel food acceptance differed by age (F = 8.08, p = 0.01). Among toddlers, greater FR receptive language (more food-related words understood) was associated with greater novel food acceptance (β [95% CI], 0.22g [−0.04, 0.49], p = 0.09). In younger infants, greater FR receptive language was associated with lower novel food acceptance (−0.80g [−1.53, −0.07], p = 0.03). Conclusions Receptive language facilitates children's understanding of their environment and contributes to shaping their behavior. Our preliminary findings suggest that greater FR receptive language may facilitate acceptance of novel food in toddlers. Younger infants were perceived to understand few words and may not have enough receptive language to positively influence food acceptance. Funding Sources This work was supported by the Government of Canada, as part of the Business Platform for Nutrition Research (BPNR) hosted by the Global Alliance for Improved Nutrition.


Nutrients ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 12 (10) ◽  
pp. 3065
Author(s):  
Norliza Ahmad ◽  
Zalilah Mohd Shariff ◽  
Firdaus Mukhtar ◽  
Munn-Sann Lye

The objective of this study was to evaluate the effect of a family-based intervention program (REDUCE) on children’s eating behaviors and dietary intake. A two-arm randomized controlled field trial was conducted among parents and children of 7 to 10 years old who were either overweight or obese. The intervention was conducted via face-to-face sessions and social media. The child eating behaviors were assessed using the child eating behaviors questionnaire (CEBQ), while their dietary consumption of vegetables and unhealthy snacks was assessed using a parental report of three days unweighted food. The generalized linear mixed modelling adjusted for covariates was used to estimate the intervention effects with alpha of 0.05. A total of 122 parents (91% response rate) completed this study. At the six-month post-training, there were statistically significant mean differences in the enjoyment of food (F(6481) = 4.653, p < 0.001), fruit and vegetable intake (F(6480) = 4.165, p < 0.001) and unhealthy snack intake (F(6480) = 5.062, p < 0.001) between the intervention and wait-list groups; however, it was not clinically meaningful. This study added to the body of knowledge of family-based intervention that utilized social media and assessed the effect in children’s eating behavior using the CEBQ and children’s dietary intake.


Author(s):  
Megan O. Bensignor ◽  
Rebecca L. Freese ◽  
Nancy E. Sherwood ◽  
Jerica M. Berge ◽  
Alicia Kunin-Batson ◽  
...  

Obesity ◽  
2006 ◽  
Vol 14 (11) ◽  
pp. 2026-2033 ◽  
Author(s):  
Scott W. Powers ◽  
Leigh A. Chamberlin ◽  
Kelly B. van Schaick ◽  
Susan N. Sherman ◽  
Robert C. Whitaker

2020 ◽  
Vol 26 (6) ◽  
pp. 493-502 ◽  
Author(s):  
P García-Segovia ◽  
V García Alcaraz ◽  
A Tárrega ◽  
J Martínez-Monzó

The demand for sustainable and healthy food is increasing. Therefore, it is necessary to find new sources of functional ingredients for design of novel food products. Microalgae are reliable sources of functional ingredients, ready for exploitation with purpose of production of human food. This work explores consumers’ acceptance of novel foods based on microalgae. To achieve natural and realistic eating location, sensory analysis was conducted in a real restaurant in Universitat Politècnica de València. A check-all-that-apply questionnaire and hedonic scale registered the consumers’ expectations ( N = 85), perceptions and acceptance, before and after taste of typical Valencian breadsticks. Food neophobia can affect acceptability of novel foods, thus, participants completed a test designed to measure their attitudes towards new food. Microalgae breadsticks have distinctive characteristics compared to control breadsticks, such as colour, flavour and odour, because of microalgae presence. Still, those breadsticks were as acceptable as the control breadsticks according to the consumer preferences. Consumers expected more differences amongst both breadsticks before tasting, but they lowered the differences in perception after trying them. Consumers consider that the product is healthier, and they would understand if it had greater expense. We believe this information can be useful for selling / marketing this novel product.


Nutrients ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 12 (11) ◽  
pp. 3409
Author(s):  
Meera D. Patel ◽  
Sharon M. Donovan ◽  
Soo-Yeun Lee

Children are often categorized as picky eaters by parents and caregivers for their rejection of foods, such as vegetables, and for exhibiting other difficult mealtime behaviors. However, there are several factors that contribute to these mealtime behaviors, including early feeding practices (i.e., breastfeeding, introduction to solid food), repeated exposure to novel foods, and genetic taste sensitivity to certain compounds. Using the online database of PubMed, a review of the literature on the development of picky eating in children, its outcomes, and intervention strategies was conducted. This review groups the developmental contributors to picky eating into the categories of nature and nurture and explores the interaction between the two. This paper will also summarize the potential outcomes of picky eating and the various strategies that are currently recommended to mitigate picky eating in young children. However, there is a lack of longitudinal work targeting consistent picky eating behaviors that have the potential to impact long-term food preferences and dietary variety. Future intervention strategies should address the factors that influence the development of picky eating on an individual level.


2019 ◽  
Vol 3 (Supplement_1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Sean Logie ◽  
Courtney Gengler ◽  
Lesley Wofford ◽  
Emily Hecht

Abstract Objectives The purpose of this quality improvement project was to implement a group exposure therapy intervention to more efficiently serve the patients that are referred to our hospital due to severe food selectivity. Methods The intervention used evidenced-based practices such as systematic desensitization, a token economy, counterconditioning, and parent training to help patients expand their food repertoire. The intervention is a 12-week multi-family group therapy for children that range in age from 4–14. Patients are placed into groups of no more than six members of similar age. In 11 of the sessions, therapists direct patients through an exposure hierarchy with novel foods and record the number of bites patients take of each food. In 4–5 sessions, a psychologist meets with parents separately to discuss how to use exposure therapy to treat food selectivity. We examined data from the groups that ran from March 2016 to June 2018. Results Using a Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks Test, we compared the number of bites of novel foods that patients took in the first and last food exposure sessions. Results indicated that patients took significantly more bites of novel foods in the last group (median = 34) compared to the first group (median = 8.5), z = 4.79, P < .001. Conclusions The outcomes of this project suggest that time-limited group interventions may help children who struggle with severe food selectivity. Time-limited groups also may also decrease treatment wait time for patients with severe food selectivity because patients can be seen simultaneously and with clear start and end date for the service. Funding Sources N/A. Supporting Tables, Images and/or Graphs


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document