scholarly journals Getting hooked: the role of a U-shaped body chamber in the shell of adult heteromorph ammonites

2014 ◽  
Vol 80 (4) ◽  
pp. 354-364 ◽  
Author(s):  
Alexander I. Arkhipkin
Keyword(s):  
1962 ◽  
Vol 203 (1) ◽  
pp. 49-52 ◽  
Author(s):  
Arthur T. Skarin ◽  
Marilyn A. Lockwood ◽  
Fred R. Griffith

Intravenous infusion of epinephrine and norepinephrine into eviscerated cats at rates of 4 µg/kg/min for 5 min results in an increase in oxygen consumption to an average 121% and 123% of control, respectively, with return to control level in the 15-min period after infusion. A similar response is obtained in normal cats during epinephrine infusion, but only in this group is a secondary or delayed rise to an average 110% of control noted in the 15-min period after infusion. It is suggested that the calorigenic action of epinephrine might be separated into an immediate effect occurring during intravenous infusion, independent of the presence of the liver, and a delayed or secondary effect occurring after infusion, requiring the presence of the liver. Both drugs change lung volume and thus introduce artifacts that interfere with measurement of oxygen consumption by conventional methods. This is overcome by use of a thermostat-controlled compensating body chamber which frees the oxygen consumption record from any dependence on lung volume.


Author(s):  
Jenny Lu ◽  
Elena A. Westeinde ◽  
Lydia Hamburg ◽  
Paul M. Dawson ◽  
Cheng Lyu ◽  
...  

When an animal moves through the world, its brain receives a stream of information about the body's translational movement. These incoming movement signals, relayed from sensory organs or as copies of motor commands, are referenced relative to the body. Ultimately, such body-centric movement signals must be transformed into world-centric coordinates for navigation [1]. Here we show that this computation occurs in the fan-shaped body in the Drosophila brain. We identify two cell types in the fan-shaped body, PFNd and PFNv [2,3], that conjunctively encode translational velocity signals and heading signals in walking flies. Specifically, PFNd and PFNv neurons form a Cartesian representation of body-centric translational velocity — acquired from premotor brain regions [4,5] — that is layered onto a world-centric heading representation inherited from upstream compass neurons [6-8]. Then, we demonstrate that the next network layer, comprising hΔB neurons, is wired so as to transform the representation of translational velocity from body-centric to world-centric coordinates. We show that this transformation is predicted by a computational model derived directly from electron microscopy connectomic data [9]. The model illustrates the key role of a specific network motif, whereby the PFN neurons that synapse onto the same hΔB neuron have heading-tuning differences that offset the differences in their preferred body-centric directions of movement. By integrating a world-centric representation of travel velocity over time, it should be possible for the brain to form a working memory of the path traveled through the environment [10-12].


JAMA ◽  
1966 ◽  
Vol 195 (12) ◽  
pp. 1005-1009 ◽  
Author(s):  
D. J. Fernbach
Keyword(s):  

JAMA ◽  
1966 ◽  
Vol 195 (3) ◽  
pp. 167-172 ◽  
Author(s):  
T. E. Van Metre

2018 ◽  
Vol 41 ◽  
Author(s):  
Winnifred R. Louis ◽  
Craig McGarty ◽  
Emma F. Thomas ◽  
Catherine E. Amiot ◽  
Fathali M. Moghaddam

AbstractWhitehouse adapts insights from evolutionary anthropology to interpret extreme self-sacrifice through the concept of identity fusion. The model neglects the role of normative systems in shaping behaviors, especially in relation to violent extremism. In peaceful groups, increasing fusion will actually decrease extremism. Groups collectively appraise threats and opportunities, actively debate action options, and rarely choose violence toward self or others.


2018 ◽  
Vol 41 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kevin Arceneaux

AbstractIntuitions guide decision-making, and looking to the evolutionary history of humans illuminates why some behavioral responses are more intuitive than others. Yet a place remains for cognitive processes to second-guess intuitive responses – that is, to be reflective – and individual differences abound in automatic, intuitive processing as well.


2020 ◽  
Vol 43 ◽  
Author(s):  
Stefen Beeler-Duden ◽  
Meltem Yucel ◽  
Amrisha Vaish

Abstract Tomasello offers a compelling account of the emergence of humans’ sense of obligation. We suggest that more needs to be said about the role of affect in the creation of obligations. We also argue that positive emotions such as gratitude evolved to encourage individuals to fulfill cooperative obligations without the negative quality that Tomasello proposes is inherent in obligations.


2020 ◽  
Vol 43 ◽  
Author(s):  
Andrew Whiten

Abstract The authors do the field of cultural evolution a service by exploring the role of non-social cognition in human cumulative technological culture, truly neglected in comparison with socio-cognitive abilities frequently assumed to be the primary drivers. Some specifics of their delineation of the critical factors are problematic, however. I highlight recent chimpanzee–human comparative findings that should help refine such analyses.


2020 ◽  
Vol 43 ◽  
Author(s):  
Thomas Parr

Abstract This commentary focuses upon the relationship between two themes in the target article: the ways in which a Markov blanket may be defined and the role of precision and salience in mediating the interactions between what is internal and external to a system. These each rest upon the different perspectives we might take while “choosing” a Markov blanket.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document