TENURE RELATED CHANGES IN WILD THOMAS'S LANGURS II: LOUD CALLS

Behaviour ◽  
1999 ◽  
Vol 136 (5) ◽  
pp. 627-650 ◽  
Author(s):  
Peter Assink ◽  
Serge Wich ◽  
Romy Steenbeek

AbstractIn this paper, we investigate how the number and context of male loud calls of Thomas's langurs (Presbytis thomasi) change over a male's tenure, in relation to changes in the intensity of male mate competition and relative male strength. We also investigate how the calls' impact on the behavior of receivers varies over tenure phases. Thomas's langurs live in one-male multi-female groups; only males produce loud calls; both males and females disperse from their natal groups; female secondary dispersal is also common, and infanticide occurs. The life-span of a group is, as a rule, restricted to the tenure of its reproductive male (median tenure length is 72 months). Male tenure in bisexual groups was divided into three phases: the early phase (no infants yet), the stable middle phase, and the late phase (last year). Because AMBs remained after all females had left a male, they were treated as a fourth phase. We hypothesised that the tendency to answer another male's calls decreases with distance because a male will invest less when answering becomes less relevant. The tendency to respond to a loud call by an extra-group male indeed decreased with distance, which suggests that males invested less in (costly) calling behavior when the chance of an interaction with that male was low. Extra-group males seemed to recognise males of new groups: they did not discriminate between medium and far distances in answering calls from (relatively unfamiliar) early tenure males. We further hypothesised that an increase in male mate competition would result in more call bouts per day and a higher tendency to answer calls, which was not found. Males with a relatively low strength were expected to keep signalling their presence, but because this low strength includes a higher risk for females and infants, we expected females to avoid loud calling extra-group males. Males with a declining strength continued to signal their presence, as was expected, but they did reduce participation in dawn call bouts, which might be a particularly sensitive measure of their decreased strength. Extra-group males answered calls by males during their late tenure phase more often at medium and far distances, which shows that males recognised calls from late tenure males. Females' avoidance of calling extra-group males remained constant during the early and middle phase but increased during the late tenure phase, as was expected. AMB males clearly avoided males from bisexual groups: they never participated in dawn call bouts, they rarely started or answered calls and they travelled away from calling males. AMB males only answered a call bout in the case of a betweengroup conflict, when their position was already known. Hence, in Thomas's langurs, loud call behavior influenced male mate competition, and it varied in relation to changes in relative male strength.

Author(s):  
Young-Min Han ◽  
Min Sun Kim ◽  
Juyeong Jo ◽  
Daiha Shin ◽  
Seung-Hae Kwon ◽  
...  

AbstractThe fine-tuning of neuroinflammation is crucial for brain homeostasis as well as its immune response. The transcription factor, nuclear factor-κ-B (NFκB) is a key inflammatory player that is antagonized via anti-inflammatory actions exerted by the glucocorticoid receptor (GR). However, technical limitations have restricted our understanding of how GR is involved in the dynamics of NFκB in vivo. In this study, we used an improved lentiviral-based reporter to elucidate the time course of NFκB and GR activities during behavioral changes from sickness to depression induced by a systemic lipopolysaccharide challenge. The trajectory of NFκB activity established a behavioral basis for the NFκB signal transition involved in three phases, sickness-early-phase, normal-middle-phase, and depressive-like-late-phase. The temporal shift in brain GR activity was differentially involved in the transition of NFκB signals during the normal and depressive-like phases. The middle-phase GR effectively inhibited NFκB in a glucocorticoid-dependent manner, but the late-phase GR had no inhibitory action. Furthermore, we revealed the cryptic role of basal GR activity in the early NFκB signal transition, as evidenced by the fact that blocking GR activity with RU486 led to early depressive-like episodes through the emergence of the brain NFκB activity. These results highlight the inhibitory action of GR on NFκB by the basal and activated hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA)-axis during body-to-brain inflammatory spread, providing clues about molecular mechanisms underlying systemic inflammation caused by such as COVID-19 infection, leading to depression.


1993 ◽  
Vol 71 (1) ◽  
pp. 76-82 ◽  
Author(s):  
David L. Gibo ◽  
Jody A. McCurdy

The migration of Danaus plexippus during the late summer in southern Ontario in 1986 lasted for about 8 weeks and consisted of three phases, an early phase characterized by increasing abundance, a middle phase of peak abundance, and a late phase characterized by declining abundance. As the season progressed, systematic changes were observed in wet mass, dry mass, lean dry mass, lipid mass, and forewing length. Wet mass, lean dry mass, and forewing length were similar for early- and middle-phase individuals, but declined in late-phase migrants. Lipid mass peaked in the middle phase of the migration and then declined abruptly in the late phase. Dry mass also peaked in the middle phase, reflecting changes in lipid mass and lean dry mass. We hypothesize that the observed changes in lipid mass and lean dry mass over the 8 weeks resulted from changes in population structure as well as seasonal changes in the weather, and in availability of nectar. Opposing conclusions reached in previous studies of lipid accumulation in D. plexippus are probably the result of failure to control for phase of migration.


1969 ◽  
Vol 6 (4) ◽  
pp. 289-308 ◽  
Author(s):  
G. R. Johnson ◽  
A. Koestner ◽  
O. Kindig ◽  
J. A. Shadduck

The effects of a pathogenic canine herpesvirus were studied sequentially in established canine thyroid adenocarcinoma tissue culture cells. The lesions were defined as early, middle, and late manifestations of viral effects based on the time a lesion first appeared. The early phase (3 to 10 hours) consisted of nucleolar swelling followed by disruption. The nucleolar changes were characterized by loss of RNA as determined with acridine orange and segregation of the nucleolar components as demonstrated by electron microscopy. The middle phase (10 to 34 hours) was characterized by the appearance of three types of eosinophilic intranuclear inclusions. One type of inclusion appeared to transform progressively into another. The appearance of viral antigen in the nucleus as demonstrated by immunofluorescence, correlated with ultrastructural evidence of viral replication. The late phase (34 to 72 hours) was manifested by cellular degeneration and viral release into the cytoplasm.


Behaviour ◽  
2004 ◽  
Vol 141 (1) ◽  
pp. 41-51 ◽  
Author(s):  
◽  
◽  

AbstractThe ability to differentiate among calls from different individuals has been shown for a number of animal species and several functions have been suggested. One hypothesis, developed for lions (Panthera leo), is that the ability to distinguish between calls from neighbour versus strange males is linked to the avoidance of infanticidal ( i.e. strange) males. Since infanticide is widespread among primates, we tested the applicability of this hypothesis to Thomas langurs (Presbytis thomasi). Young adult males, that mostly reside in all-male bands or are solitary (called AMB males), were more likely to be infanticidal than adult, usually older, males that reside in mixed-sex groups (called MSG males). We use playbacks to demonstrate that Thomas langurs are able to differentiate between loud calls from AMB male strangers and MSG male strangers. Thomas langur males responded more vigorously to playbacks of calls from AMB (i.e. more likely to be infanticidal) males than to calls from MSG males. Females showed a more cautious response to the calls from AMB males than to the calls from MSG males. Both these reactions are in accordance with the infanticide hypothesis and suggest that Thomas langurs differentiate between loud calls of AMB and MSG stranger males and incorporate this information in their behaviour to avoid infanticide.


10.4312/dp.9 ◽  
2018 ◽  
Vol 44 ◽  
pp. 152
Author(s):  
Evgenia Leonidovna Lychagina ◽  
Aleksandr Alexeevich Vybornov

The concept of the Kama Neolithic culture was proposed by Otto Bader, but lacked ra­diocarbon dates in the 20th century. Now, we have more than 50 radiocarbon dates that can be at­tributed to the Kama Neolithic culture. The results of radiocarbon analysis of organogenic materials of the Kama culture allow us to determine its chronological limits between the second quarter of the 6th and the beginning of 4th mill. cal BC. The early phase of the Kama culture is now dated between the second quarter of the 6th and the beginning of the 5th mill. cal BC, the middle phase is dated to the first half of the 5th mill. cal BC, and the late phase is dated between the second half of 5th and the beginning of 4th mill. cal BC.


2018 ◽  
Vol 56 ◽  
pp. 35-48
Author(s):  
Antonín Přichystal

Compared with Poland, the territory of Bohemia and Moravia is not so rich in natural occurrences of high-quality siliceous rocks (silicites, ‘flints’). This contribution follows distribution of the four most attractive Polish chipped raw materials (silicite of the Cracow-Częstochowa Jurassic, ‘chocolate’ silicite, banded Krzemionki [striped] silicite and spotted Świeciechów [grey white-spotted] silicite) in the Czech Republic. Since the middle phase of Upper Palaeolithic (Gravettian) the Jurasssic-Cracow silicites had been transported to Moravia and since its late phase (Magdalenian) also to Bohemia. The first use of the ‘chocolate’ silicite has been ascertained at some Late Aurignacian (Epiaurignacian) sites of central Moravia similarly as an exceptional find attesting early use of Świeciechów spotted silicite (Late Szeletian?). No finds of the banded Krzemionki silicite have been registered in Pre-Neolithic flaked assemblages in the Czech Republic. Evidence of systematic and mass transport of silicites from the Cracow-Częstochowa Jurassic to northern/central Moravia and to eastern/central Bohemia has been found in some periods of the Neolithic (especially connected with the Linear Pottery culture). For the period of the earlier Eneolithic (Funnel Beaker culture) we can identify a small but systematic presence of raw materials from the northern foreland of the Świętokrzyskie (Holy Cross) Mountains, this comprises objects of banded Krzemionki silicite and spotted Świeciechów silicite. About 24 Moravian non-stratified finds of axes made of the banded Krzemionki silicite and polished over the whole surface can be probably connected with the Globular Amphora culture. Silicites from the Cracow-Częstochowa Jurassic appeared again in the late Eneolithic, especially as arrowheads of the Bell Beaker culture in Moravia. Only two pieces made from the Jurassic Cracow-Częstochowa silicite appeared in a collection of 1463 artefacts connected with the Early Bronze Age in Moravia


Behaviour ◽  
1999 ◽  
Vol 136 (5) ◽  
pp. 595-625 ◽  
Author(s):  
Romy Steenbeek

AbstractBetween-group conflicts are believed to serve two major functions: mate defence and resource defence. In addition, in species where females can disperse, both female choice and male coercion may play a role in between-group interactions. In this paper, I evaluate the resource vs mate defence hypotheses and investigate male and female strategies in between-group interactions in wild Thomas's langurs (Presbytis thomasi). This species lends itself well to study the intertwining of male and female strategies. Thomas's langurs live in one-male multifemale groups; both males and females disperse from their natal groups; female secondary dispersal is also common, and infanticide occurs. The life-span of a group is, as a rule, restricted to the tenure of its reproductive male (median tenure length is 72 months). Male tenure in bisexual groups was divided into three phases: the early phase (no infants yet), the stable middle phase, and the late phase (last year). Because AMBs remained after all females had left a male, they were treated as a fourth phase. I analysed interactions of group members with individuals outside the group using data from a four year field study (1093 observation days) of 15 bisexual and eight all-male groups of Thomas's langurs at Ketambe, Indonesia. Two types of interactions could be distinguished: (1) group encounters: whole groups meet each other, and (2) male provocations: a male silently approaches a group and suddenly attacks the individuals. This study involves 329 group encounters and 265 male provocations. The results support the mate defence hypothesis, but are ambiguous about the role of resource competition: Group encounters during the middle and late tenure phase took place more often in the context 'food patch', than was expected, and they mostly took place in fruit patches, which were a preferred food item. However, the proportion of group encounters that involved aggression did not depend on feeding context or tenure, and fruit availability did not influence group encounter rates. Male aggression during between-group conflicts reflected mate defence, rather than resource defence, although females had the possibility of obtaining resource defence through a male's mate defence. This study suggests that females use the outcome of group encounters and male provocations to assess the relative strength of males, and thus 'chose' a male who is able to defend future offspring. It also suggests that males can use coercion (infanticide and aggression against females) to show the relative weakness of a female's current male and encourage her to transfer: Aggressive behaviour during group encounters was primarily between males. Females only reacted aggressively to extra-group males when their infant was under attack. Male infanticide only occurred during provocations by males from neighbouring groups or AMBs. The intensity of male competition for mates, as measured by male-male aggression and extra-group male interest in a groups' females, was higher during the early and late phases, than during the middle phase of the tenure. It was also higher during the AMB than during the late phase of male tenure. The relative strength of males, as measured by a male's provoking behaviour and his ability to protect his females from aggression by extra-group males, was lower during the late than during the middle tenure phase. Male strength seemed lowest during the subsequent AMB phase. Females avoided extra-group males least during the early tenure phase, before infants were born.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document