Relationship between Manual Midline Crossing and Hand Preference

1987 ◽  
Vol 7 (3) ◽  
pp. 131-145 ◽  
Author(s):  
Janet M. Stilwell

The relationship between manual midline crossing and hand preference was studied in 200 2- to 6-year-old children. Performance patterns of right-, left-, and mixed-handed children were compared. Right- and left-handed children exhibited significantly greater frequency of preferred than nonpreferred hand use for total hand use and for contralateral hand responses. The right-handed group showed a developmental trend for contralateral hand responses, with age-related changes due to a progressive increase in frequency of contralateral right (preferred) hand responses. The mixed-handed group displayed a weak preference toward use of the right hand for total hand use and for contralateral hand responses. A general reduction in frequency of midline crossing cited within the mixed-handed group was due to reduced frequency of contralateral right-hand responses. The theoretical significance of the research findings and their implications for sensory integration test interpretation are discussed.

2011 ◽  
Vol 111 (6) ◽  
pp. 1606-1613 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ying Ye ◽  
Michael J. Griffin

Vibration of one hand reduces blood flow in the exposed hand and in the contralateral hand not exposed to vibration, but the mechanisms involved are not understood. This study investigated whether vibration-induced reductions in finger blood flow are associated with vibrotactile perception thresholds mediated by the Pacinian channel and considered sex differences in both vibration thresholds and vibration-induced changes in digital circulation. With force and vibration applied to the thenar eminence of the right hand, finger blood flow and finger skin temperature were measured in the middle fingers of both hands at 30-s intervals during seven successive 4-min periods: 1) pre-exposure with no force or vibration, 2) pre-exposure with force, 3) vibration 1, 4) rest with force, 5) vibration 2, 6) postexposure with force, and 7) recovery with no force or vibration. A 2-N force was applied during periods 2–6 and 125-Hz vibration at 0.5 and 1.5 ms−2root mean square (r.m.s.; unweighted) was applied during periods 3 and 5, respectively. Vibrotactile thresholds were measured at the thenar eminence of right hand using the same force, contact conditions, and vibration frequency. When the vibration magnitude was greater than individual vibration thresholds, changes in finger blood flow were correlated with thresholds (with both 0.5 and 1.5 ms−2r.m.s. vibration): subjects with lower thresholds showed greater reductions in finger blood flow. Women had lower vibrotactile thresholds and showed greater vibration-induced reductions in finger blood flow. It is concluded that mechanoreceptors responsible for mediating vibration perception are involved in the vascular response to vibration.


1995 ◽  
Vol 80 (2) ◽  
pp. 671-674 ◽  
Author(s):  
Carl Gabbard ◽  
Susan Hart

Prior research has shown that right-handed adults perform better on a speed-tapping task with the right hand and right foot, while left-handers execute more rapidly with the left hand and right foot. Speculation is that environmental influence, most likely driving experience, may account for the right-foot bias. To examine this hypothesis further, 48 young right- and left-handed children were tested on a similar protocol. Analyses indicated no significant differences in foot performance within hand-preference groups. Since these findings do not complement reports for adults, factors such as experience or maturation might contribute to the difference. Were patterns similar, the effect of environmental influence would be assumed to be small. However, much more evidence is needed before an adequate explanation can be developed. The issue of possible environmental influence is discussed from various theoretical perspectives.


2007 ◽  
Vol 101 (3) ◽  
pp. 185-197 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sotaro Kita ◽  
Olivier de Condappa ◽  
Christine Mohr
Keyword(s):  

Symmetry ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 13 (10) ◽  
pp. 1776
Author(s):  
Elena Nicoladis ◽  
Safi Shirazi

Previous studies have shown that gestures are mediated by the left hemisphere. The primary purpose of this study was to test whether most gestures are also asymmetrical, i.e., produced with the right hand. We also tested four predictors of the degree of right-hand gesture use: bilingualism, language ability, sex, and age. These factors have been related to differences in the degree of language lateralization. English monolinguals, French–English bilinguals, and French monolinguals watched a cartoon and told the story back. For the gestures they produced while speaking, we calculated the percentage produced with the right hand. As predicted, the majority of gestures were right-handed (60%). Bilingualism, language ability, and age were not significantly related to hand choice in either English or French. In English, males tended to produce more right-handed gestures than females. These results raise doubts as to whether hand preference in gestures reflects speech lateralization. We discuss possible alternative explanations for a right-hand preference.


1994 ◽  
Vol 72 (4) ◽  
pp. 1596-1610 ◽  
Author(s):  
A. M. Gordon ◽  
A. Casabona ◽  
J. F. Soechting

1. Experienced typists typed phrases containing words in which one isolated letter was typed with one hand, while the remaining letters were typed with the contralateral hand. 2. The translational and rotational motion of the fingers and wrist of the right hand were obtained optoelectronically from the location of reflective markers placed on the fingers. 3. Midway through the experiment, the key corresponding to the isolated letter was physically switched with another key on the keyboard, and subjects typed the letter in its new location (for 140 trials). The letter “n,” typed with the right index finger, was either switched with letters normally typed with the same finger (u), with a different finger but same hand (o), with the same finger of the left hand (v), or with a different finger of the left hand (w). 4. When the words were typed normally, the interkey intervals were relatively short, and the onset of movement of the right hand began before the preceding keypress with the left hand. Thus the movement of the two hands overlapped. Furthermore, the movement to the isolated key was highly stereotypical, with little trial-to-trial variability. 5. After the transposition of keys, there were prolongations in the interkey intervals, with the largest delay occurring directly before the typing of the transposed key. Switches between homologous fingers (involving mirror movements) delayed the onset of keypresses to a lesser extent than did other switches. With practice, these delays were reduced but never reached the control level. 6. After the keyswitch, the onset of movement to the isolated key did not occur on average until after the last keypress with the contralateral hand, except when the switch involved the use of homologous fingers. In the latter case, overlapping movement of the two hands was maintained. Thus the learning of a series of discrete movements does not necessarily require that each movement segment be performed sequentially. 7. After the transposition of keys, the movement pattern and time course to a given key were similar to the movement patterns for that key observed during control trials in all conditions. Thus the learning of a series of movements may involve the use of previously learned movements under new conditions. 8. The results suggest that typing movements may be organized at several levels, including the individual keystroke and word level.


2006 ◽  
Vol 6 ◽  
pp. 1805-1809 ◽  
Author(s):  
Victor Frak ◽  
D. Bourbonnais ◽  
I. Croteau ◽  
H. Cohen

One the most fundamental aspects of the human motor system is the hemispheric asymmetry seen in behavioral specialization. Hemispheric dominance can be inferred by a contralateral hand preference in grasping. Few studies have considered grasp orientation in the context of manual lateralization and none has looked at grasp orientation with natural prehension. Thirty right-handed adults performed precision grasps of a cylinder using the thumb and index fingers, and the opposition axis (OA) was defined as the line connecting these two contact points on the cylinder. Subjects made ten consecutive grasps with one hand (primary hand movements) followed by ten grasps with the other hand (trailing movements). Differences between primary and trailing grasps revealed that each hemisphere is capable of programming the orientation of the OA and that primary movements with the right hand significantly influenced OA orientation of the trailing left hand. These results extend the hemispheric dominance of the left hemisphere to the final positions of fingers during prehension.


2021 ◽  
Vol 24 (1) ◽  
pp. 25-34
Author(s):  
Alexandr B. Krivosheev ◽  
Tamara V. Ermachenko ◽  
Pavel P. Khavin ◽  
Inga A. Krivosheeva ◽  
Dmitry V. Morozov

Lyells syndrome is rare. Up to 10 cases are registered per million population per year. The mortality rate for Lyells syndrome is 512%. Two patients were observed aged, respectively, 38 years and 72 years with Lyells syndrome. The disease manifested itself against the background of local application of drugs. In one case ― Dolobens gel as an analgesic at the site of a bruise of the right hand. In another case, a 20% solution of chlorohxidin biogluconate as a mouth rinse for stomatitis. The first symptoms of the disease occurred locally at the site of application of these medicines. In the first case, the skin of the right hand, in the second ― the mucous membrane of the mouth. The outcome of the disease depended on early diagnosis, urgent hospitalization in a specialized department, from the area of skin lesions, the appointment of adequate therapy. Lyells syndrome can occur at any age, and the risk of developing the disease increases in those over the age of 40. In the elderly, SL in terms of the first symptoms and rate of disease progression, the nature of skin and mucous membrane lesions does not have any fundamental differences with those in younger patients. The distinctive features and risk factors of Lyells syndrome in the elderly include, firstly, its occurrence against the background of the already formed comorbid age-related pathology of internal organs, secondly, the concomitant age-related pathology during the development of an acute toxic-allergic reaction is decompensated, a multiorgan failure is formed, which is the cause of more severe Lyells syndrome and higher mortality. In young people, a comorbid viral infection can be considered a risk factor, especially the combination of HIV-infection and chronic hepatitis C.


1995 ◽  
Vol 80 (3) ◽  
pp. 979-994 ◽  
Author(s):  
Syoichi Iwasaki ◽  
Takehito Kaiho ◽  
Ken Iseki

Hand-preference data of 2316 Japanese were analyzed by age groups, sex, and familial sinistrality. Right-hand preference increased across age groups at least up to 30 years for men, while women showed relatively stable and stronger preference for right-hand use. Unlike some Western studies, no linear trends across age groups were found for both sexes. Declining cultural censorship against left-handedness would not be responsible for the trends, since there was no evidence indicating such a decline in Japan. Hypotheses of reduced longevity and life-long adaptation to the right-handed world are not satisfactory either, since both hypotheses assume a linear trend spanning the entire life span. Thus, it seems that a single-factor hypothesis which explains all the results by resorting to a single cause does not account for the complex results found in this and other studies.


2005 ◽  
Vol 99 (4) ◽  
pp. 1483-1493 ◽  
Author(s):  
Martin V. Sale ◽  
John G. Semmler

The purpose of the study was to examine age-related differences in electromyographic (EMG) responses to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) during functional isometric contractions in left and right hands. EMG responses were recorded from the first dorsal interosseus muscle following TMS in 10 young (26.6 ± 1.3 yr) and 10 old (67.6 ± 2.3 yr) right-handed subjects. Muscle evoked potentials (MEPs) and silent-period durations were obtained in the left and right hands during index finger abduction, a precision grip, a power grip, and a scissor grip, while EMG was held constant at 5% of maximum. For all tasks, MEP area was 30% ( P < 0.001) lower in the left hand of old compared with young subjects, whereas there was no age difference in the right hand. The duration of the EMG silent period was 14% ( P < 0.001) shorter in old (150.3 ± 2.9 ms) compared with young (173.9 ± 3.0 ms) subjects, and the age differences were accentuated in the left hand (19% shorter, P < 0.001). For all subjects, the largest MEP area (10–12% larger) and longest EMG silent period (8–19 ms longer) were observed for the scissor grip compared with the other three tasks, and the largest task-dependent change in these variables was observed in the right hand of older adults. These differences in corticospinal control in the left and right hands of older adults may reflect neural adaptations that occur throughout a lifetime of preferential hand use for skilled (dominant) and unskilled (nondominant) motor tasks.


2017 ◽  
Vol 30 (5) ◽  
pp. 490-495 ◽  
Author(s):  
Banu Alicioglu ◽  
Guliz Yilmaz ◽  
Ozgur Tosun ◽  
Nail Bulakbasi

Recent studies have pointed out dysfunction and histopathological changes of the choroid plexuses (CPs) with aging. This paper reviews apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC) values of the CPs for age-related changes. All the brain MR images of the patients between January 2013 and June 2014 in our Radiology Department were retrospectively investigated. Patients with major cranial abnormalities (brain tumors, hyperacute or acute ischemia, developmental anomalies, hemorrhage, hydrocephaly) were excluded. Diffusion-weighted images were obtained at the parameter values of b = 1000 s/mm2 in the axial plane. The transverse diameters of the lateral ventricles (LVs) and ADC values of both CPs were measured. Brain MRIs of 202 individuals, 97 men (48%), 105 women (52%), were studied. There were statistically significant positive correlations between the ADC values of CP and patient ages. (Right CP: r = 0.623; p < 0.05. Left CP: r = 0.654; p < 0.05). There were positive correlations between LV diameters and age ( r = 0.624, p < 0.05 for the right LV; r = 0.621, p < 0.05 for the left LV). The ADC values of age groups significantly differed ( p < 0.05); the ≥61-year-old group was significantly higher compared to younger individuals. There is a progressive increase of water diffusivity in the CPs during aging. ADC values should be considered as a neuroimaging quantitative biomarker in normal aging-dementia syndromes.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document