Impact of plumb location on the determination accuracy of chimney heeling by photographic method

2017 ◽  
Vol 919 (1) ◽  
pp. 55-59
Author(s):  
O.V. Raskatkina

There is a method of using the corded plumb as vertical reference straight line, located in front of the objective of a digital photocamera in the article. When we take picture of the object under study, there will be this straight line in the photo, from which we can carry out all necessary measurements in the Paint system with the following conversion them into metric system. All possible variants of location of the reference straight line relative to it axis are considered by the example of the construction of the tower round shape and it is shown a method of heeling calculation by image processing results. Experimental research to determine the degree of influence of plumb location in the photo relative to it axe on the accuracy of the heeling determination was carried out by shooting the brick chimney with the 30 metres height when the plumb is located on the chimney axis and on different distance from the left and right of the axis. It is set in the result that the plumb location has influence on the accuracy of heeling determination. The optimal location is on the centre of the top section of the chimney and there is shown the method of accounting corrections due to inaccurate location.

2019 ◽  
Vol 121 (4) ◽  
pp. 1342-1351 ◽  
Author(s):  
Keaton Proud ◽  
James B. Heald ◽  
James N. Ingram ◽  
Jason P. Gallivan ◽  
Daniel M. Wolpert ◽  
...  

Skillful manipulation requires forming and recalling memories of the dynamics of objects linking applied force to motion. It has been assumed that such memories are associated with entire objects. However, we often control different locations on an object, and these locations may be associated with different dynamics. We have previously demonstrated that multiple memories can be formed when participants are explicitly instructed to control different visual points marked on an object. A key question is whether this novel finding generalizes to more natural situations in which control points are implicitly defined by the task. To answer this question, we used objects with no explicit control points and tasks designed to encourage the use of distinct implicit control points. Participants moved a handle, attached to a robotic interface, to control the position of a rectangular object (“eraser”) in the horizontal plane. Participants were required to move the eraser straight ahead to wipe away a column of dots (“dust”), located to either the left or right. We found that participants adapted to opposing dynamics when linked to the left and right dust locations, even though the movements required for these two contexts were the same. Control conditions showed this learning could not be accounted for by contextual cues or the fact that the task goal required moving in a straight line. These results suggest that people naturally control different locations on manipulated objects depending on the task context and that doing so affords the formation of separate motor memories. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Skilled manipulation requires forming motor memories of object dynamics, which have been assumed to be associated with entire objects. However, we recently demonstrated that people can form multiple memories when explicitly instructed to control different visual points on an object. In this article we show that this novel finding generalizes to more natural situations in which control points are implicitly defined by the task.


Author(s):  
A.E. Trejo ◽  
M.-C. Jung ◽  
M.S. Hallbeck

As part of a continuous effort of reaching the optimal use of the intuitool, a study was conducted to identify the optimal diameter combination between the trackball and the end effector ball. The task was to simulate the end effector movement during an operation, using different diameter combinations. Twenty students performed the trackball-controlling tasks to move the end effector from an initial position to designated circular-shaped targets. The trackball diameters were 19 mm and 40 mm, and those of the end effector balls were 3 mm, 5 mm, and 10 mm. There were four targets: right, left, up, and down. Travel time, accuracy, and trail deviation were measured as independent variables. Accuracy was not a significant factor showing that all participants followed instructions to reach each target as accurately as possible. The time to reach the target depended both on target location and trackball to end effector ratio individually and in their interaction. It was quickest to get to the upper target compared to all other locations. Trial deviation depended only on the target position and the target location and ratio interaction. The performance of going in a straight line was best for the left and right directions as opposed to up and down using the trackball.


2012 ◽  
Vol 31 (3) ◽  
pp. 83-87 ◽  
Author(s):  
Birutė Ruzgienė ◽  
Wolfgang Förstner

Up-to-date digital photogrammetry involves operations on huge data sets, and with classical image processing procedures it might be time consuming to find out the best solution. One of the key tasks is to detect outliers in given data, eg for curve fitting or image matching. The problem is hard as the number of outliers is usually large, possibly larger than 50%, thus powerful estimation techniques are needed. We demonstrate one of these techniques, namely Random Sample Consensus (RANSAC), for fitting a model to sample data, especially for fitting a straight line through a set of given points. Experiments with up to 80% outliers prove the efficiency of RANSAC. The results are representative for image analysis in digital photogrammetry


2021 ◽  
Vol 4 (1) ◽  
pp. 74-80
Author(s):  
Murat Silahtaroğlu ◽  
Serkan Dereli

Drowsiness is one of the underlying causes of driving accidents leading to serious injuries and deaths annually. According to the experts has mentioned that almost 30% of all traffic accidents have been caused by drowsiness. In avoiding these traffic accidents, a proper system is required to prevent the driver from falling asleep. This study proposes a real-time image processing-based system for recognizing the drowsiness face expression of the vehicle driver. The method of this study, detecting the exact position facial landmarks and both left and right eyes using dlib and eye aspect ratio algorithm. This system, after detecting drowsiness eye, give audible alert the vehicle driver to stay awake throughout the driving journey.


2014 ◽  
Vol 1037 ◽  
pp. 71-74
Author(s):  
Tao Hua ◽  
Zheng Gang Lu

In order to help researchers better understand the dynamic characteristic of independent wheelset rail vehicles, this article presents a new modeling way to establish an independent wheelset bogie with a white-box dynamic analysis tool-MapleSim. In this article, an independent wheelset model is made firstly. Than based on this wheelset model, an independent wheelset bogie is made. After the modeling, the necessary validation with Simpack is done. The straight line dynamic validation includes the creepage of left and right wheels, the lateral displacements of bogie and both wheelsets’ yaw and lateral displacements. The results are similar to the model with the same parameters in Simpack which reveals that the bogie model in this article is correct and feasible.


1979 ◽  
Vol 34 (5) ◽  
pp. 648-652
Author(s):  
K. Ueda ◽  
G. Lehmpfuhl ◽  
E. Reuber

Abstract An accurate photographic method for measuring intensities on a fluorescent screen is described. Simultaneously with the fluorescent screen a calibrated 7-step grey scale is recorded on the film. Using a scanning photometer the intensity distribution can be digitized and by comparison with the grey scale converted to electron current density. As an example LEED-intensities from a Si (lll)-surface obtained with the photographic technique are compared with spot-photometer measurements.


Procedia CIRP ◽  
2016 ◽  
Vol 41 ◽  
pp. 777-782 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kanuengnit Patoommakesorn ◽  
Frédéric Vignat ◽  
François Villeneuve

PeerJ ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 8 ◽  
pp. e10354
Author(s):  
Agneta Egenvall ◽  
Hanna Engström ◽  
Anna Byström

Background Biomechanical studies of walk, especially walk on the circle, are scarce, while circles or curved tracks are frequently used during equestrian activities. To study horse–rider–circle interactions on the circle, the first steps would be to investigate how the unridden, freely walking horse is influenced by circular movement, and then add a rider. The aim was to study horse vertical trunk movements, and sagittal cannon angles (protraction–retraction) during walk in straight-line and on the circle without rider, and on the circle with a rider using minimal influence. Methods Ten horses were ridden by five riders, summing to 14 trials. Each trial included straight walk unridden (on concrete), and walk on 10 m diameter circles (left and right on soft surface) first lunged (unridden) and then ridden with minimal rider influence. Inertial measurement units (100 Hz) were positioned on the withers, third sacral vertebra (S3) and laterally on metacarpal and metatarsal bones (using self-adhesive bandage). Selected data were split in steps (withers and S3 vertical translations) or strides (cannon protraction–retraction) at maximum hind limb protraction, and range of motion (ROM), minima and maxima, and their timing, were extracted. Data were analyzed using mixed models with inner/outer/straight nested within unridden/ridden as fixed effect, and controlling for stride duration. Differences between: inner vs outer steps/limbs; the same step/limb unridden vs ridden; and the same step/limb straight vs inner/outer unridden; were examined for statistical significance at p < 0.05. Results Inner limbs had smaller cannon ROM than outer limbs, for example, forelimbs when ridden (inner vs outer 62° vs 63°) and hind limbs when unridden (53° vs 56°). Forelimb cannon ROM was the largest for straight (65°). Hind limb ROM for straight walk (55°) was in-between inner (52–53°) and outer hind limbs (56–57°). Vertical ROM of S3 was larger during the inner (unridden/ridden 0.050/0.052 m) vs the outer step (unridden/ridden 0.049/0.051 m). Inner (0.050 m) and outer steps (0.049 m) unridden had smaller S3 ROM compared to straight steps (unridden, 0.054 m). Compared to when unridden, withers ROM was smaller when ridden: inner hind steps unridden/ridden 0.020 vs 0.015 m and outer hind steps 0.020 vs 0.013 m. When ridden, withers ROM was larger during the inner hind step vs the outer. Conclusion The outer hind limb had greater cannon pro-retraction ROM, compared to the inner limb. Larger croup ROM during the inner step appears to be coupled to increased retraction of the outer hind limb. Knowledge of magnitudes and timing of the horse’s movements on the circle in unridden and ridden walk may stimulate riders to educate eye and feel in analyzing the execution of circles, and stimulate further studies of the walk, for example, on interactions with rider influence, natural horse asymmetries, or lameness.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document