vocal responses
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

149
(FIVE YEARS 24)

H-INDEX

28
(FIVE YEARS 2)

2021 ◽  
pp. 168-174
Author(s):  
Mary T Black

Verbal imagery in the context of the choral rehearsal is explored in this chapter in order to understand how it is employed and what its effects are. Choral directors employ imagery to communicate to singers how to create and change vocal responses. Imagery is allied to specific features in the music and enables singers to gain conceptual understanding of aspects of the vocal sound. The study is based on research into the contexts and efficacy of verbal imagery in choral rehearsals; vocal responses were examined and imagery’s efficacy in affecting all categories of sound was observed, with examples demonstrating the perceived effects on tonal quality. Implications for directors highlight the efficacy of verbal imagery in assisting directors to achieve their musical and creative goals. Directors are encouraged to be creative in devising and employing imagery during their interactions with singers, enabling singers to create the desired vocal responses.


Animals ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 11 (10) ◽  
pp. 2763
Author(s):  
Meredith Sheil ◽  
Giulia Maria De Benedictis ◽  
Annalisa Scollo ◽  
Suzanne Metcalfe ◽  
Giles Innocent ◽  
...  

Piglet castration results in acute pain and stress to the animal. There is a critical need for effective on-farm methods of pain mitigation. Local anaesthesia using Tri-Solfen® (Animal Ethics Pty Ltd., Melbourne, Australia), a topical local anaesthetic and antiseptic formulation instilled to the wound during surgery, is a newly evolving on-farm method to mitigate castration pain. To investigate the efficacy of Tri-Solfen®, instilled to the wound during the procedure, to alleviate subsequent castration-related pain in neonatal piglets, we performed a large, negatively controlled, randomised field trial in two commercial pig farms in Europe. Piglets (173) were enrolled and randomised to undergo castration with or without Tri-Solfen®, instilled to the wound immediately following skin incision. A 30 s wait period was then observed prior to completing castration. Efficacy was investigated by measuring pain-induced motor and vocal responses during the subsequent procedure and post-operative pain-related behaviour in treated versus untreated piglets. There was a significant reduction in nociceptive motor and vocal response during castration and in the post-operative pain-related behaviour response in Tri-Solfen®-treated compared to untreated piglets, in the first 30 min following castration. Although not addressing pain of skin incision, Tri-Solfen® is effective to mitigate subsequent acute castration-related pain in piglets under commercial production conditions.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Michelle E.H. Fournet ◽  
Leanna P. Matthews ◽  
Annie Bartlett ◽  
Natalie Mastick ◽  
Fred Sharpe ◽  
...  

AbstractHumpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) produce calls across age and sex class and throughout their migratory range. Despite growing interest in calling behavior, the function of most calls is unknown. Among identified call types, the ‘whup’ is ubiquitous, and innate, and may serve as a contact call. We conducted an acoustic playback experiment combined with passive acoustic monitoring and visual observations to test the function of the whup on a Southeast Alaskan foraging ground. Using a before-during-after design, we broadcasted either a control sound or a unique whup call sequence. We investigated the change in whup rates (whups/whale/10 minutes) in response to treatment (whup or control) and period (before, during, or after). In 100% of the conspecific trials, whup rates increased during broadcasts, and whup rates were significantly higher than in before or after periods. There was no significant difference in whup rates between before and after periods during conspecific trials. In control trials, there were no significant differences in whup rates between before, during, or after periods. Neither whups nor control playbacks elicited an approach response. Humpback whale vocal responses to whup playbacks suggest that whups function as a contact call, but not necessarily as an aggregation signal.


2021 ◽  
Vol 15 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mohammad Seidisarouei ◽  
Sander van Gurp ◽  
Nicole Melisa Pranic ◽  
Irina Noguer Calabus ◽  
Marijn van Wingerden ◽  
...  

Social animals tend to possess an elaborate vocal communication repertoire, and rats are no exception. Rats utilize ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) to communicate information about a wide range of socially relevant cues, as well as information regarding the valence of the behavior and/or surrounding environment. Both quantitative and qualitative acoustic properties of these USVs are thought to communicate context-specific information to conspecifics. Rat USVs have been broadly categorized into 22 and 50 kHz call categories, which can be further classified into subtypes based on their sonographic features. Recent research indicates that the 50 kHz calls and their various subtype profiles may be related to the processing of social and non-social rewards. However, only a handful of studies have investigated USV elicitation in the context of both social and non-social rewards. Here, we employ a novel behavioral paradigm, the social-sucrose preference test, that allowed us to measure rats’ vocal responses to both non-social (i.e., 2, 5, and 10% sucrose) and social reward (interact with a Juvenile rat), presented concurrently. We analyzed adult male Long-Evans rats’ vocal responses toward social and non-social rewards, with a specific focus on 50 kHz calls and their 14 subtypes. We demonstrate that rats’ preference and their vocal responses toward a social reward were both influenced by the concentration of the non-social reward in the maze. In other words, rats showed a trade-off between time spent with non-social or social stimuli along with increasing concentrations of sucrose, and also, we found a clear difference in the emission of flat and frequency-modulated calls in the social and non-social reward zones. Furthermore, we report that the proportion of individual subtypes of 50 kHz calls, as well as the total USV counts, showed variation across different types of rewards as well. Our findings provide a thorough overview of rat vocal responses toward non-social and social rewards and are a clear depiction of the variability in the rat vocalization repertoire, establishing the role of call subtypes as key players driving context-specific vocal responses of rats.


2021 ◽  
Vol 55 (2) ◽  
Author(s):  
Martin A. Schlaepfer ◽  
Dennis Caldwell ◽  
James C. Rorabaugh ◽  
Michael J. Ryan ◽  
Shaylon Stump ◽  
...  
Keyword(s):  

2021 ◽  
Vol 18 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Ke Deng ◽  
Ya Zhou ◽  
Qiao-Ling He ◽  
Bi-Cheng Zhu ◽  
Tong-Liang Wang ◽  
...  

Abstract Background Signal detection is crucial to survival and successful reproduction, and animals often modify behavioral decisions based on information they obtained from the social context. Undeniably, the decision-making in male-male competition and female choice of anurans (frogs and toads) depends heavily on acoustic signals. However, increasing empirical evidence suggests that additional or alternative types of cue (e.g., visual, chemical, and vibratory) can be used to detect, discriminate and locate conspecifics in many anuran species. Nevertheless, few studies have investigated whether conspecific odor cues affect male’s calling behavior. In this study, we conducted an experiment to investigate whether and how different chemical cues (male odors, female odors, and stress odors) from conspecifics affect male’s calling strategies in serrate-legged small treefrogs (Kurixalus odontotarsus), and whether the combined chemical and acoustic stimuli have additive effects on calling behavior or not. Results We found that compared with female odors, male K. odontotarsus reduced calling investment in response to male odors or stress odors, in the absence of rival’s advertisement calls. When odor stimuli and advertisement calls were presented simultaneously, however, there were no differences in the vocal response of focal males among odor groups. Conclusions These results provide evidence that male treefrogs switch calling investment according to different odor cues from conspecifics, and further demonstrate that calling behavior can be affected by chemical cues in anuran species. Our study highlights the potential role of airborne chemical cues in sex identification and contributes to increase our understanding of anuran communication.


The Auk ◽  
2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Emma I Greig ◽  
Eva Kinnebrew ◽  
Max L Witynski ◽  
Eric C Larsen

Abstract Most birds that show geographic variation in their songs discriminate between local and foreign songs, which may help them avoid unnecessary conflicts with vagrant individuals or similar-sounding congeners. However, some species respond equally to foreign and local songs, which may be useful if foreign individuals present territorial threats or if there are no sympatric congeners to avoid. Species without sympatric congeners are not commonly tested in playback studies, but they offer an opportunity to see how song variation and recognition unfolds when the pressure to avoid similar congeners is absent. Here, we use Verdins (Auriparus flaviceps), a monotypic genus of songbird with no confamilials in North America, to explore song variation and recognition in a species living without close relatives. We assessed geographic variation in song across the Verdin range and conducted a playback experiment using exemplars from 2 acoustically divergent and geographically distant regions as treatments. We found significant geographic variation in song that mapped well onto ecologically distinct desert regions. We found that Verdins had stronger vocal responses to local-sounding songs, but had equal movement responses to local-sounding and foreign songs. These results are similar to results found in other species without sympatric congeners and provide an example of a species that investigates acoustically divergent conspecific songs, despite recognizing salient differences in those songs.


2021 ◽  
Vol 49 (1) ◽  
pp. 150-158
Author(s):  
Michael S. Osmanski ◽  
Yoshimasa Seki ◽  
Robert J. Dooling

AbstractBudgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) are small Australian parrots with a well-documented, learned vocal repertoire and a high degree of vocal production learning. These birds live in large, social flocks and they vocally interact with each other in a dynamic, reciprocal manner. We assume that budgerigars must process and integrate a wide variety of sensory stimuli when selecting appropriate vocal responses to conspecifics during vocal interactions, but the relative contributions of these different stimuli to that process are next to impossible to tease apart in a natural context. Here we show that budgerigars, under operant control, can learn to respond to specific stimuli with a specific vocal response. Budgerigars were trained to produce contact calls to a combination of auditory and visual cues. Birds learned to produce specific contact calls to stimuli that differed either in location (visual or auditory) or quality (visual). Interestingly, the birds could not learn to associate different vocal responses with different auditory stimuli coming from the same location. Surprisingly, this was so even when the auditory stimuli and the responses were the same (i.e., the bird’s own contact call). These results show that even in a highly controlled operant context, acoustic cues alone were not sufficient to support vocal production learning in budgerigars. From a different perspective, these results highlight the significant role that social interaction likely plays in vocal production learning so elegantly shown by Irene Pepperberg’s work in parrots.


Science ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 371 (6528) ◽  
pp. 503-507 ◽  
Author(s):  
Alison J. Barker ◽  
Grigorii Veviurko ◽  
Nigel C. Bennett ◽  
Daniel W. Hart ◽  
Lina Mograby ◽  
...  

Naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber) form some of the most cooperative groups in the animal kingdom, living in multigenerational colonies under the control of a single breeding queen. Yet how they maintain this highly organized social structure is unknown. Here we show that the most common naked mole-rat vocalization, the soft chirp, is used to transmit information about group membership, creating distinctive colony dialects. Audio playback experiments demonstrate that individuals make preferential vocal responses to home colony dialects. Pups fostered in foreign colonies in early postnatal life learn the vocal dialect of their adoptive colonies, which suggests vertical transmission and flexibility of vocal signatures. Dialect integrity is partly controlled by the queen: Dialect cohesiveness decreases with queen loss and remerges only with the ascendance of a new queen.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document