scholarly journals Behavioral responses to physical vs. social novelty in male and female laboratory rats

2011 ◽  
Vol 88 (1) ◽  
pp. 56-59 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sonia A. Cavigelli ◽  
Kerry C. Michael ◽  
Sheila G. West ◽  
Laura Cousino Klein
1981 ◽  
Vol 7 (6) ◽  
pp. 315-320 ◽  
Author(s):  
L. Valzelli ◽  
S. Bernasconi ◽  
S. Garattini

2013 ◽  
Vol 5 (3) ◽  
pp. 186-194 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hella Bartsch ◽  
Dieter Mecke ◽  
Hansgeorg Probst ◽  
Heinz Kupper ◽  
Eckhard Seebald ◽  
...  

1976 ◽  
Vol 38 (3) ◽  
pp. 959-965 ◽  
Author(s):  
Larry M. Dean ◽  
Frank N. Willis ◽  
James M. La Rocco

Reactions to the invasion of personal space in terms of age, sex, and race of the invaders were investigated. Children, grouped by sex (male and female), race (black and white), and age (5, 8, and 10 yr. old), invaded the personal space of 192 adults grouped by sex (male and female) and race (black and white). The six types of behavioral responses were: avoidance, aggression, exploratory behavior, facilitative behavior, excess motor activity, and failure to respond. Responses to personal space invasion were not affected by sex. Blacks responded more often than whites but did not differ with regard to any particular type of behavioral response. Age of the invader had a significant effect on type of response given by adults whose personal space was invaded. It was concluded that the age of the invader was much more important than race or sex in determining the response to invasion of personal space.


2021 ◽  
pp. 104474
Author(s):  
Thiele P. Souza ◽  
Francini Franscescon ◽  
Flavia V. Stefanello ◽  
Talise E. Müller ◽  
Laura W. Santos ◽  
...  

2005 ◽  
Vol 47 (5) ◽  
pp. 620-628 ◽  
Author(s):  
C. Westenbroek ◽  
T.A.B. Snijders ◽  
J.A. den Boer ◽  
M. Gerrits ◽  
D.S. Fokkema ◽  
...  

2019 ◽  
Vol 5 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Michael H Parsons ◽  
Michael A Deutsch ◽  
Dani Dumitriu ◽  
Jason Munshi-South

Abstract Wild rats (Rattus norvegicus) are among the most ubiquitous and consequential organisms in the urban environment. However, collecting data from city rats is difficult, and there has been little research to determine the influence, or valence, of rat scents on urban conspecifics. Using a mark-release-monitor protocol, we previously learned rats can be attracted to remote-sensing points when baited with mixed-bedding from male and female laboratory rats. It was thus essential that we disambiguate which scents were eliciting attraction (+ valence), inspection, a conditioned response whereby attraction may be followed by avoidance (–valence), or null-response (0 valence). We used radio-frequency identification tagging and scent-baited antennas to assess extended (>40 days) responses to either male or female scents against two risk presentations (near-shelter and exposed to predators). In response to male scents, rats (n = 8) visited both treatments (shelter, exposed) more than controls (0.2 visits/day treatment vs. 0.1/day; P < 0.05) indicating scents accounted for response more so than risk. Dwell-times, however, did not differ (1.2 s/visit treatment vs. 0.9 s/visit; P > 0.5). These outcomes are consistent with inspection (–valence). In response to female scents, rats (n = 7) increased visitation (5.02 visits/day vs. 0.1/day controls; P < 0.05), while dwell-times also increased 6.8 s/visit vs. 0.2 s/visit in both risk-settings. The latter is consistent with persistent attraction (+valence), but was also influenced by shelter, as runway visits (1.1 visits/day) were a magnitude more common than predator-exposed (0.1 visits/day). Further understanding and exploiting the mobility of city rats is necessary for improvements in basic and applied research, including city pathogen-surveillance and urban wildlife management.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document