Properties of odour plumes from natural baits

2011 ◽  
Vol 110 (3) ◽  
pp. 459-464 ◽  
Author(s):  
Håkan Westerberg ◽  
Karin Westerberg
Keyword(s):  
2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
K. J. Painter ◽  
A. Z. Plochocka

AbstractA dot in the vastness of the Atlantic, Ascension Island remains a lifelong goal for the green sea turtles that hatched there, returning as adults every three or four years to nest. This navigating puzzle was brought to the scientific community’s attention by Charles Darwin and remains a topic of considerable speculation. Various cues have been suggested, with orientation to geomagnetic field elements and following odour plumes to their island source among the most compelling. Via a comprehensivein silicoinvestigation we test the hypothesis that multimodal cue following, in which turtles utilise multiple guidance cues, is the most effective strategy. Specifically, we combine agent-based and continuous-level modelling to simulate displaced virtual turtles as they attempt to return to the island. Our analysis shows how population homing efficiency improves as the number of utilised cues is increased, even under “extreme” scenarios where the overall strength of navigating information decreases. Beyond the paradigm case of green turtles returning to Ascension Island, we believe this could commonly apply throughout animal navigation.


1999 ◽  
Vol 202 (12) ◽  
pp. 1639-1648 ◽  
Author(s):  
M. Geier ◽  
O.J. Bosch ◽  
J. Boeckh

Both the concentration and the fine-scale plume structure of host odours influence the upwind flight of female mosquitoes Aedes aegypti (L.) (Diptera: Culicidae) in a wind tunnel. The attractive effects of carbon dioxide, human skin odour and l-(+)-lactic acid were tested in homogeneous, turbulent and filamentous odour plumes. With carbon dioxide, the percentage of upwind-flying mosquitoes increased with the increasing fluctuations in concentration that occur in turbulent and filamentous plumes. In homogeneous plumes, an initial activation effect was observed, but sustained upwind flights were less frequent than in the other plumes. The opposite was found with plumes of human skin odour: the highest number of mosquitoes flew upwind in the homogeneous plume, whereas in turbulent or filamentous plumes their numbers were significantly lower. Regardless of plume type, the percentage of upwind-flying mosquitoes increased with increasing concentrations of carbon dioxide and of skin odour. With l-(+)-lactic acid, the dose-response characteristics were not consistent, and the relative effects of different plume types upon upwind flights differed within different ranges of concentration. Even maximum reactions to this compound were modest compared with those to carbon dioxide or to skin odour. Our findings demonstrate (1) that mosquitoes are able to orient upwind under continuous odour stimulation and (2) that upwind flight is dependent upon plume structure in different ways for different host odour components.


1993 ◽  
Vol 83 (3) ◽  
pp. 329-333 ◽  
Author(s):  
John Brady ◽  
Nigel Griffiths

AbstractVideo observations were made of tsetse flies (mainly Glossina pallidipes Austen) as they approached, responded to and left a vertical (1 m square) black target in odour plumes of acetone, or a 4:1:8 mixture of octenol:propylphenol: methylphenol, or acetone plus this “4:1:8” mixture, or in no odour. No differences in mean flight speed or turn size in any of these situations were detected. With the odour source 5 m upwind of the target, the flight tracks of tsetse arriving at and leaving the target were significantly biased towards upwind, highly so when 4:1:8 was present, marginally so in acetone alone. With the source 10 m upwind, the same biases were still present but weaker. Circling flights around the black target were more frequent in acetone plus 4:1:8 than in no odour (26% vs 15%), but in either odour alone were only just significantly more than in no odour. Upwind turning at the target was more frequent (25% vs 17%) in acetone alone that in no odour (though not in 4:1:8 alone). It is concluded that 4:1:8 elicits an upwind anemotactic response comparable in strength to that in CO2, and that acetone elicits a similar response more weakly, but may be more involved in potentiating visual responses.


2013 ◽  
Vol 89 (1) ◽  
pp. 68-81 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ivo Beyaert ◽  
Monika Hilker

1984 ◽  
Vol 74 (1) ◽  
pp. 143-152 ◽  
Author(s):  
G. A. Vale

AbstractIn the Zambezi Valley of Zimbabwe, the numbers of Glossina morsitans morsitans Westw. and G. pallidipes Aust. electrocuted as they arrived near a visual target were increased five times by the release of a mixture of carbon dioxide and acetone vapour at the target. Catches declined to near the no-odour level as the odours were moved to release points 32–40 m upwind or downwind of the baits. This pattern of catches at the target changed when flies were trapped at the odour release points 4–40 m from the target, and changed when the odour was released at each of several points along the axis of the wind instead of at a single point. These changes suggest that the flies responded to a single-point release of the odours at distances of 30–60 m from the release point, that, when the flies arrived at the upwind end of a plume and discovered no visual bait, they flew upwind for a few metres and returned downwind for about 8 m and that the flies navigated effectively up a composite plume made by 2–33 separate release points, 1–8 m apart, along a line up to 30° from the mean direction of the wind. Data for Muscidae are also presented.


2004 ◽  
Vol 82 (10) ◽  
pp. 1547-1553 ◽  
Author(s):  
David Drolet ◽  
John H Himmelman

We performed a factorial experiment to investigate the effects of current and prey odours (mussels) on the displacement behaviour of the sea star Asterias vulgaris (Verrill, 1866). This sea star is a common subtidal predator of sessile and slow-moving animals in the western North Atlantic Ocean. In the presence of current and prey odours in a flume, sea stars oriented themselves upstream and 70% succeeded in finding the prey. Also, the degree of orientation toward the prey increased as the sea star approached the prey. In contrast, only 5% of individuals tested in still water found the prey. Thus, for A. vulgaris the presence of macroscale flow is an essential condition for locating distant prey. Sea stars tested in current alone showed rheotactic behaviour, moving diagonally upstream. This behaviour should enhance the probability of encountering prey odour plumes in the field. Sea stars moved faster and straighter in flowing water than in still water. The slow movement of A. vulgaris in still water probably minimizes costs of foraging when there is a low chance of finding prey and the straight diagonal movement in current should ensure that the sea star continuously samples new areas, rather than resampling the same odour-free area.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document