How unitary is the capacity-limited attentional focus?

2001 ◽  
Vol 24 (1) ◽  
pp. 146-147 ◽  
Author(s):  
Torsten Schubert ◽  
Peter A. Frensch

Cowan assumes a unitary capacity-limited attentional focus. We argue that two main problems need to be solved before this assumption can complement theoretical knowledge about human cognition. First, it needs to be clarified what exactly the nature of the elements (chunks) within the attentional focus is. Second, an elaborated process model needs to be developed and testable assumptions about the proposed capacity limitation need to be formulated.

2020 ◽  
Vol 29 (1) ◽  
pp. 39-56
Author(s):  
Eric Rundquist

Cognitive Grammar analyses the semantics of linguistic features in relation to human cognition; Free Indirect Style allows authors to represent their characters’ cognition with language. This article applies Cognitive Grammar to the analysis of a character’s mind that is represented with Free Indirect Style. In the tradition of mind style analysis, it aims to use linguistics to reveal some of the underlying cognitive processes and proclivities at work in the character’s psychology. The character in question is the protagonist in Malcolm Lowry’s Under the Volcano, an alcoholic who is largely characterised by his drunken behaviour and ideation. This article therefore focuses on the linguistic features that serve to represent his inebriated state of mind. It analyses the semantic effects of those features primarily in terms of attentional focus, drawing on Cognitive Grammar concepts, such as objective construal, specificity, scope, profile and domain, and relating these to the protagonist’s cognitive proclivities for solipsism, partial awareness, delayed reaction, attenuated experience and self-delusion. The article also discusses the theoretical background for mind style analysis, arguing for the continued importance of focusing on the relationship between the text and a character’s mind, alongside the focus on the reader’s mind that has come to dominate cognitive stylistics.


Author(s):  
Charles Beck

An integrative, systems-based model of knowledge sharing can provide a way of visualizing the interrelated elements that comprise a knowledge management system. This original model, building on a rhetorical process model of communication, includes both the objective and subjective elements within the human cognition. In addition, it clarifies the purpose and method elements at the center for any effective knowledge system. The model centers on the purpose elements of intentions and audience, and the method elements of technical tools and human processes. The output of knowledge sharing includes objective products and subjective interpretations. Feedback verifies the timeliness and efficiency in the process of building both information and knowledge.


2018 ◽  
Vol 24 (1) ◽  
pp. 198-205 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christian D. Helfrich ◽  
Adam J. Rose ◽  
Christine W. Hartmann ◽  
Leti Bodegom‐Vos ◽  
Ian D. Graham ◽  
...  

2017 ◽  
Author(s):  
Falk Lieder ◽  
Tom Griffiths ◽  
Quentin J.M. Huys ◽  
Noah D. Goodman

Cognitive biases, such as the anchoring bias, pose a serious challenge to rational accounts of human cognition. We investigate whether rational theories can meet this challenge by taking into account the mind’s bounded cognitive resources. We asked what reasoning under uncertainty would look like if people made rational use of their finite time and limited cognitive resources. To answer this question, we applied a mathematical theory of bounded rationality to the problem of numerical estimation. Our analysis led to a rational process model that can be interpreted in terms of anchoring-and-adjustment. This model provided a unifying explanation for ten anchoring phenomena including the differential effect of accuracy motivation on the bias towards provided versus self-generated anchors. Our results illustrate the potential of resource-rational analysis to provide formal theories that can unify a wide range of empirical results and reconcile the impressive capacities of the human mind with its apparently irrational cognitive biases.


2009 ◽  
pp. 1967-1979
Author(s):  
Charles E. Beck

An integrative, systems-based model of knowledge sharing can provide a way of visualizing the interrelated elements that comprise a knowledge management system. This original model, building on a rhetorical process model of communication, includes both the objective and subjective elements within the human cognition. In addition, it clarifies the purpose and method elements at the center for any effective knowledge system. The model centers on the purpose elements of intentions and audience, and the method elements of technical tools and human processes. The output of knowledge sharing includes objective products and subjective interpretations. Feedback verifies the timeliness and efficiency in the process of building both information and knowledge.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Roberta Rocca

The vast majority of everyday social practices involves a form of joint interaction with the environment which relies on establishing a shared attentional focus (Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, & Moll, 2005). Languages mediate humans’ ability to coordinate attention via a specific class of words: spatial demonstratives. Words like the pronouns “this” or “that”, or the adverbs “here” and “there” are among the few undisputed language universals (Diessel, 1999, 2014). They are developmental (Capirci, Iverson, Pizzuto, & Volterra, 1996) and evolutionary (Diessel, 2006; 2013) cornerstones of language, and they are among the most frequent words in the lexicon (Leech & Rayson, 2014). Demonstratives are deictic expressions (from Ancient Greek deixis, “demonstration, indication”). They can in principle be used to indicate any object, and their meaning depends on the context of utterance (H. H. Clark & Bangerter, 2004; Diessel, 1999; S. C. Levinson, 1983a, 2004). Identifying their referent thus hinges on the availability of information on the perceptual context (which objects are perceptually available), multimodal cues (pointing gestures, gaze; Clark & Bangerter, 2004; Cooperrider, 2016; García, Ehlers, & Tylén, 2017; Stevens & Zhang, 2013), expectations (which objects are relevant for the present interaction; what the speaker may intend to refer to; Clark, 1996; Levinson, 1983) and cues provided by the use of specific demonstrative forms (e.g. a proximal “this” vs. a distal “that”). Yet, in spite of their semantically underspecified nature, these expressions function as powerful and effective coordination devices for social interaction. But what are the neural and cognitive mechanisms that enable the integration of linguistic, perceptual and pragmatic information required for the comprehension of demonstratives? Which cues on the intended referent does the use of proximal versus distal demonstrative forms actually provide? And finally, how can demonstratives function as effective tools for social interaction, in spite of their semantic vagueness? In the present thesis, I report three studies where these questions were addressed using novel experimental paradigms. The results of the three studies provide novel insights on the neural and cognitive underpinnings of demonstratives, as well as their key function in social interaction. However, their scope goes beyond an understanding of demonstratives per se. First, knowledge on the neural substrates of spatial demonstratives is informative with respect to the bigger question of how the brain extracts meaning from linguistic input. Secondly, our findings provide general insights on the relationship between language processing and extralinguistic cognition, highlighting its tight link to perception and attention (Article 1), to functional, action-oriented representations of the physical world (Article 2), and the role of partner-oriented adaptations of language use in successful social coordination (Article 3). Finally, this dissertation contributes to the development of new experimental methods for further research using demonstratives as a searchlight into core aspects of human cognition, and it outlines concrete suggestions in this direction.


2020 ◽  
Vol 43 ◽  
Author(s):  
Charles P. Davis ◽  
Gerry T. M. Altmann ◽  
Eiling Yee

Abstract Gilead et al.'s approach to human cognition places abstraction and prediction at the heart of “mental travel” under a “representational diversity” perspective that embraces foundational concepts in cognitive science. But, it gives insufficient credit to the possibility that the process of abstraction produces a gradient, and underestimates the importance of a highly influential domain in predictive cognition: language, and related, the emergence of experientially based structure through time.


2020 ◽  
Vol 43 ◽  
Author(s):  
Aba Szollosi ◽  
Ben R. Newell

Abstract The purpose of human cognition depends on the problem people try to solve. Defining the purpose is difficult, because people seem capable of representing problems in an infinite number of ways. The way in which the function of cognition develops needs to be central to our theories.


2019 ◽  
Vol 42 ◽  
Author(s):  
Daniel J. Povinelli ◽  
Gabrielle C. Glorioso ◽  
Shannon L. Kuznar ◽  
Mateja Pavlic

Abstract Hoerl and McCormack demonstrate that although animals possess a sophisticated temporal updating system, there is no evidence that they also possess a temporal reasoning system. This important case study is directly related to the broader claim that although animals are manifestly capable of first-order (perceptually-based) relational reasoning, they lack the capacity for higher-order, role-based relational reasoning. We argue this distinction applies to all domains of cognition.


1979 ◽  
Vol 44 (1) ◽  
pp. 3-30 ◽  
Author(s):  
Carol A. Pruning

A rationale for the application of a stage process model for the language-disordered child is presented. The major behaviors of the communicative system (pragmatic-semantic-syntactic-phonological) are summarized and organized in stages from pre-linguistic to the adult level. The article provides clinicians with guidelines, based on complexity, for the content and sequencing of communicative behaviors to be used in planning remedial programs.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document