scholarly journals Cohesion in male singing behavior predicts group reproductive output in a social songbird

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ammon Perkes ◽  
Marc F Schmidt ◽  
H. Luke Anderson Anderson ◽  
Julie Gros-Louis ◽  
David White

All social groups require organization to function optimally. Group organization is often shaped by social 'rules', which function to manage conflict, discourage cheating, or promote cooperation. If social rules promote effective social living, then the ability to learn and follow these rules may be expected to influence individual and group-level fitness. However, such links can rarely be tested, due to the complexity of the factors mediating social systems and the difficulty of gathering data across multiple groups. Songbirds offer an opportunity to investigate the link between social rules and reproductive output because most of their social interactions are mediated by song, a well-studied and readily quantifiable behavior. Using observations from 19 groups of brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater) studied across 15 years, we find evidence for a previously undocumented social rule: cohesive group transitions between dominance- and courtship-related singing. Comparing across groups, the degree of cohesion in male singing behavior predicts the reproductive output of their group. Experimental manipulation of group structure via the introduction of juvenile males to captive flocks reduced group cohesion and adult male reproductive success. Taken together, these results demonstrate that cohesion in group behavioral states can affect both individual and group-level reproductive success, suggesting that selection can act not only on individual-level traits, but also on an individual's ability and opportunity to participate effectively in organized social interactions. Social cohesion could therefore be an unappreciated force affecting social evolution in many diverse systems.

2020 ◽  
Vol 287 (1931) ◽  
pp. 20201026
Author(s):  
Robin E. Morrison ◽  
Winnie Eckardt ◽  
Tara S. Stoinski ◽  
Lauren J. N. Brent

Social complexity reflects the intricate patterns of social interactions in societies. Understanding social complexity is fundamental for studying the evolution of diverse social systems and the cognitive innovations used to cope with the demands of social life. Social complexity has been predominantly quantified by social unit size, but newer measures of social complexity reflect the diversity of relationships. However, the association between these two sets of measures remains unclear. We used 12 years of data on 13 gorilla groups to investigate how measures of social complexity relate to each other. We found that group size was a poor proxy for relationship diversity and that the social complexity individuals experienced within the same group varied greatly. Our findings demonstrate two fundamental takeaways: first, that the number of relationships and the diversity of those relationships represent separate components of social complexity, both of which should be accounted for; and second, that social complexity measured at the group level may not represent the social complexity experienced by individuals in those groups. These findings suggest that comprehensive studies of social complexity, particularly those relating to the social demands faced by individuals, may require fine-scale social data to allow accurate comparisons across populations and species.


Behaviour ◽  
1990 ◽  
Vol 113 (1-2) ◽  
pp. 57-80 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christopher W. Petersen

AbstractThe hermaphroditic reef fish Serranus fasciatus exhibits three types of social systems. The size of a social group is correlated with the local density of conspecifics. At very low densities, isolated pairs of individuals reciprocally spawn with each other, achieving equal current reproductive success. At intermediate group sizes, harems form, with the largest individual typically losing all of its female function and becoming a functional male. In harems, subordinate hermaphrodites obtain little male reproductive success through streaking, an alternative male mating tactic. The lone pure male maintains almost total monopolization of male reproductive success in harems, apparently due to aggressive domination of subordinates. At high group sizes, the ability of the male to monopolize all of the matings in a social group decreases, and some of the larger hermaphrodites obtain some male-role reproductive success by pair spawning with smaller subordinate hermaphrodites while continuing to spawn as females with the male. Mating partners stay relatively constant through time, resulting in a pattern of small 'sub-harems' within harems. These mating tactics are consistent with the hypothesis that dominant individuals increase their current reproductive success in this species by restricting male mating opportunities of conspecifics. Subordinate individuals spawn as males when the dominant is unable to restrict interactions between hermaphrodites that are potential mates, or when they successfully streak. The increased male reproductive success of hermaphrodites in isolated pairs and complex harems compared with hermaphrodites in harems appears to be important in maintaining a hermaphroditic subordinate phenotype in this largely non-reciprocating species.


2009 ◽  
Vol 55 (2) ◽  
pp. 133-147 ◽  
Author(s):  
Daniele Macale ◽  
Massimiliano Scalici ◽  
Alberto Venchi

Both demography and population regulation play an important role in the theory of sustainable exploitation and conservation of threatened taxa, such as terrestrial Chelonia. Here, we show and discuss some dynamic aspects of Testudo kleinmanni using modal progression analysis of length compositions. Although the Testudinata physiology is very different from that of fish, their growth model conforms to the Von Bertalanffy growth model. We observed a maximum of three age classes for both juveniles and females, and four classes for males. No appreciable between-sex differences were found in growth patterns, except for the diverse asymptotic length. Females should be subject to a strong sexual selection to quickly reach a large size in order to optimize lifetime reproductive output. The T. kleinmanni male size could be driven by predation escape and by easy accessibility to females, rather than by fighting for them. Thus, male reproductive success increases with the ability to fertilize females and female reproductive success increases with the ability to produce eggs, creating a large divergence in the context of selection between sexes. Different selective (synergetic or antagonistic) forces would appear to favor divergence in size between sexes. Additional properties found in the study regard the elevated mortality rate, probably also due to the human impact (poaching), and the relatively high longevity (26 and 22 years for females and males, respectively). Dynamics studies are useful for planning in situ activities of monitoring the population status, and could have a role in introducting programs and in control of reintroduced individuals during a restocking project.


2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sam G. B. Roberts ◽  
Anna Roberts

Group size in primates is strongly correlated with brain size, but exactly what makes larger groups more ‘socially complex’ than smaller groups is still poorly understood. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) are among our closest living relatives and are excellent model species to investigate patterns of sociality and social complexity in primates, and to inform models of human social evolution. The aim of this paper is to propose new research frameworks, particularly the use of social network analysis, to examine how social structure differs in small, medium and large groups of chimpanzees and gorillas, to explore what makes larger groups more socially complex than smaller groups. Given a fission-fusion system is likely to have characterised hominins, a comparison of the social complexity involved in fission-fusion and more stable social systems is likely to provide important new insights into human social evolution


2020 ◽  
Vol 6 (38) ◽  
pp. eaaz5746
Author(s):  
Catherine Crockford ◽  
Liran Samuni ◽  
Linda Vigilant ◽  
Roman M. Wittig

Humans are unusual among animals for continuing to provision and care for their offspring until adulthood. This “prolonged dependency” is considered key for the evolution of other notable human traits, such as large brains, complex societies, and extended postreproductive lifespans. Prolonged dependency must therefore have evolved under conditions in which reproductive success is gained with parental investment and diminished with early parental loss. We tested this idea using data from wild chimpanzees, which have similarly extended immature years as humans and prolonged mother-offspring associations. Males who lost their mothers after weaning but before maturity began reproducing later and had lower average reproductive success. Thus, persistent mother-immature son associations seem vital for enhancing male reproductive success, although mothers barely provision sons after weaning. We posit that these associations lead to social gains, crucial for successful reproduction in complex social societies, and offer insights into the evolution of prolonged dependency.


2006 ◽  
Vol 39 (2) ◽  
pp. 287-300 ◽  
Author(s):  
MHAIRI A. GIBSON ◽  
RUTH MACE

Summary.This study examines the reproductive success of men and women in rural Ethiopia as a function of their marital status, specifically by comparing polygamously and monogamously married individuals. In line with predictions from evolutionary theory, polygamy is beneficial to male reproductive success (i.e. producing larger numbers of surviving offspring). The success of polygamously married females depends on wife rank: the first wives of polygamous husbands do better than monogamously married women and much better than second or third wives. These effects are mirrored in child nutritional status: the children of second and third wives have lower weight for height. Due to potential, largely unmeasurable differences in marriageability (quality) between individuals, it was not possible to support a model of either resource-holding polygyny combined with female choice or female coercion into unwanted marriages. First wives of polygamously married men marry at a younger age and attract a higher brideprice, suggesting that both the males and females in the marriage are likely to be of higher quality (due to wealth, family status or some other factor such as beauty). Unions that end up monogamous are likely to be between slightly lower quality individuals; and second and third wives, who marry at the oldest ages and attract the lowest brideprice, may be ‘making the best of a bad job’. The relatively long gap between first and second marriages may mean that first wives of highly marriageable males can enjoy considerable reproductive success before their husbands marry again.


Behaviour ◽  
2011 ◽  
Vol 148 (11-13) ◽  
pp. 1372-1392 ◽  
Author(s):  
Alice U. Edler ◽  
Thomas W.P. Friedl

AbstractThe role of bright plumage colouration for female choice has been the focus of research in sexual selection for many years, with several studies showing that females prefer the most elaborately ornamented males, which are often also the highest quality individuals. Here, we analysed the associations between reproductive performance and plumage, body condition and blood parasite load in the red bishop (Euplectes orix), a sexually dimorphic and polygynous weaverbird species, where males in a carotenoid-based orange-to-red breeding plumage defend territories and build many nests to which they try to attract females. Male reproductive success in terms of number of nests accepted was mainly determined by the number of nests built, but was also positively related to blood parasite load, while we found no influence of plumage characteristics. Together with previously obtained data, our results indicate that plumage characteristics in the red bishop do not affect male reproductive success and are generally not suitable to reliably indicate male quality. We suggest that the primary function of the brilliant orange-scarlet breeding plumage might be presence signalling in terms of increasing conspicuousness of breeding males to females searching for mates.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document