High utilization of inpatient psychiatric services by older adults

1997 ◽  
Vol 48 (2) ◽  
pp. 172-174 ◽  
2018 ◽  
Vol 24 (3) ◽  
pp. 188-194 ◽  
Author(s):  
Oleksandr Khrypunov ◽  
Raheel Aziz ◽  
Ban Al-Kaissy ◽  
Ketan Jethwa ◽  
Verghese Joseph

SUMMARYOlder people with mental health problems are entitled to the same level and quality of care as younger people. Several factors continue to influence policy and delivery of older adults' mental health services in the UK. Following the introduction of the Equality Act 2010, there has been a drive to create an ‘ageless’ National Health Service. This has opened up the debate about whether such a service is best equipped to meet the specific needs of older adults. In this contribution we consider the concepts of ‘old age’ and ‘frailty’ and their clinical and service provision implications in psychiatry. The management of late-life depression and early-onset dementia, advance care planning and palliation in dementia are also considered.LEARNING OBJECTIVES•Appreciate how old age psychiatric services and the concept of ‘old age’ have evolved over time•Gain an understanding of issues at the interface between old age and general adult psychiatry and those specific to old age psychiatry•Gain an overview of procedures involved in transferring care between general adult and old age psychiatric services and the need for a separate old age psychiatry subspecialtyDECLARATION OF INTERESTNone.


2011 ◽  
Vol 19 (8) ◽  
pp. 695-703 ◽  
Author(s):  
Derek D. Satre ◽  
Stacy A. Sterling ◽  
R. Scott Mackin ◽  
Constance Weisner

2020 ◽  
Vol 4 (Supplement_1) ◽  
pp. 171-172
Author(s):  
Helen-Maria Vasiliadis ◽  
Isabelle Pitrou ◽  
Djamal Berbiche

Abstract Studies that examined satisfaction with care in older adults are scarce. The aim of this research was to analyse satisfaction among older adults considering mental health, socio-clinical and health system factors and by age category. Data come from the Étude sur la Santé des Aînés Services study including 1,624 adults aged ≥65 years recruited between 2011-2013 in primary care in Quebec. Patient satisfaction was assessed during interviews with questions adapted from the Primary Care Assessment Survey. Mental health (anxiety, depression, suicidal ideation, psychological distress, cognition), social support, quality of life, the presence of pain and chronic conditions were self-reported. Health service use was extracted from administrative registries. Logistic regressions stratified by age were used to examine the associations of low satisfaction in three dimensions of care. For continuity of care, the determinants of low satisfaction were pain and attraction index for psychiatric services in adults 65 to 75 years versus anxiety, cognition and hospitalizations in adults 75 years and older. For patient-provider interactions, the determinants were psychological distress, attraction index for psychiatric services in adults 65 to 75 years versus quality of life and cognition in adults 75 years and older. For adequacy of care, anxiety, psychological distress, social support, pain, quality of life and attraction index for psychiatric services were significant in adults 65 to 75 years versus quality of life and cognition in adults 75 years and older. Results highlight different patterns of satisfaction by age category that should be used to improve care delivered in primary care.


2008 ◽  
Vol 32 (8) ◽  
pp. 298-302 ◽  
Author(s):  
Daniel Anderson ◽  
Howard Cattell ◽  
Elaine Bentley

Aims and MethodTo comprehensively describe a nurse-led consultation liaison service for older adults by retrospectively reviewing all referrals received in 2006 and comparing them against other services and benchmark reports.ResultsOf the 298 individuals referred to psychiatric services from other hospital wards, 120 were aged 85–94 years old (40%), 193 were male (65%) and 152 were referred from geriatrics (51%). A majority of 204 have not had previous contact with psychiatric services (69%). the most common diagnosis was dementia (33%, n=88), with 27% individuals (n=65) being referred onwards to secondary care.Clinical ImplicationsThis nurse-led service, using a novel approach of a support worker providing further community support, functions well compared with traditional consultation models. It helps identify many individuals with dementia and engages them into community psychiatric services.


1997 ◽  
Vol 3 (3) ◽  
pp. 138-145 ◽  
Author(s):  
Susan M. Benbow ◽  
Alison Marriott

Those who work with older adults will inevitably find themselves working with families. Indeed assessment of an older person is often difficult if members of their family and social network are not involved. Referrals to social and psychiatric services are often precipitated by family changes. Ratna & Davis (1984) described 142 consecutive referrals to a community old age psychiatry service and found retirement, family conflict, departure of or illness of a carer, or bereavement precipitated 60% of referrals. Thus, work with older adults will often necessitate work with families, although it may not be seen in those terms.


2019 ◽  
Vol 42 ◽  
Author(s):  
Colleen M. Kelley ◽  
Larry L. Jacoby

Abstract Cognitive control constrains retrieval processing and so restricts what comes to mind as input to the attribution system. We review evidence that older adults, patients with Alzheimer's disease, and people with traumatic brain injury exert less cognitive control during retrieval, and so are susceptible to memory misattributions in the form of dramatic levels of false remembering.


2019 ◽  
Vol 62 (5) ◽  
pp. 1258-1277 ◽  
Author(s):  
Megan K. MacPherson

PurposeThe aim of this study was to determine the impact of cognitive load imposed by a speech production task on the speech motor performance of healthy older and younger adults. Response inhibition, selective attention, and working memory were the primary cognitive processes of interest.MethodTwelve healthy older and 12 healthy younger adults produced multiple repetitions of 4 sentences containing an embedded Stroop task in 2 cognitive load conditions: congruent and incongruent. The incongruent condition, which required participants to suppress orthographic information to say the font colors in which color words were written, represented an increase in cognitive load relative to the congruent condition in which word text and font color matched. Kinematic measures of articulatory coordination variability and movement duration as well as a behavioral measure of sentence production accuracy were compared between groups and conditions and across 3 sentence segments (pre-, during-, and post-Stroop).ResultsIncreased cognitive load in the incongruent condition was associated with increased articulatory coordination variability and movement duration, compared to the congruent Stroop condition, for both age groups. Overall, the effect of increased cognitive load was greater for older adults than younger adults and was greatest in the portion of the sentence in which cognitive load was manipulated (during-Stroop), followed by the pre-Stroop segment. Sentence production accuracy was reduced for older adults in the incongruent condition.ConclusionsIncreased cognitive load involving response inhibition, selective attention, and working memory processes within a speech production task disrupted both the stability and timing with which speech was produced by both age groups. Older adults' speech motor performance may have been more affected due to age-related changes in cognitive and motoric functions that result in altered motor cognition.


2020 ◽  
Vol 29 (3) ◽  
pp. 391-403
Author(s):  
Dania Rishiq ◽  
Ashley Harkrider ◽  
Cary Springer ◽  
Mark Hedrick

Purpose The main purpose of this study was to evaluate aging effects on the predominantly subcortical (brainstem) encoding of the second-formant frequency transition, an essential acoustic cue for perceiving place of articulation. Method Synthetic consonant–vowel syllables varying in second-formant onset frequency (i.e., /ba/, /da/, and /ga/ stimuli) were used to elicit speech-evoked auditory brainstem responses (speech-ABRs) in 16 young adults ( M age = 21 years) and 11 older adults ( M age = 59 years). Repeated-measures mixed-model analyses of variance were performed on the latencies and amplitudes of the speech-ABR peaks. Fixed factors were phoneme (repeated measures on three levels: /b/ vs. /d/ vs. /g/) and age (two levels: young vs. older). Results Speech-ABR differences were observed between the two groups (young vs. older adults). Specifically, older listeners showed generalized amplitude reductions for onset and major peaks. Significant Phoneme × Group interactions were not observed. Conclusions Results showed aging effects in speech-ABR amplitudes that may reflect diminished subcortical encoding of consonants in older listeners. These aging effects were not phoneme dependent as observed using the statistical methods of this study.


Author(s):  
Eun Jin Paek ◽  
Si On Yoon

Purpose Speakers adjust referential expressions to the listeners' knowledge while communicating, a phenomenon called “audience design.” While individuals with Alzheimer's disease (AD) show difficulties in discourse production, it is unclear whether they exhibit preserved partner-specific audience design. The current study examined if individuals with AD demonstrate partner-specific audience design skills. Method Ten adults with mild-to-moderate AD and 12 healthy older adults performed a referential communication task with two experimenters (E1 and E2). At first, E1 and participants completed an image-sorting task, allowing them to establish shared labels. Then, during testing, both experimenters were present in the room, and participants described images to either E1 or E2 (randomly alternating). Analyses focused on the number of words participants used to describe each image and whether they reused shared labels. Results During testing, participants in both groups produced shorter descriptions when describing familiar images versus new images, demonstrating their ability to learn novel knowledge. When they described familiar images, healthy older adults modified their expressions depending on the current partner's knowledge, producing shorter expressions and more established labels for the knowledgeable partner (E1) versus the naïve partner (E2), but individuals with AD were less likely to do so. Conclusions The current study revealed that both individuals with AD and the control participants were able to acquire novel knowledge, but individuals with AD tended not to flexibly adjust expressions depending on the partner's knowledge state. Conversational inefficiency and difficulties observed in AD may, in part, stem from disrupted audience design skills.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document