scholarly journals Variable Speed Across Dimensions of Ability in the Joint Model for Responses and Response Times

2021 ◽  
Vol 12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Peida Zhan ◽  
Hong Jiao ◽  
Kaiwen Man ◽  
Wen-Chung Wang ◽  
Keren He

Working speed as a latent variable reflects a respondent’s efficiency to apply a specific skill, or a piece of knowledge to solve a problem. In this study, the common assumption of many response time models is relaxed in which respondents work with a constant speed across all test items. It is more likely that respondents work with different speed levels across items, in specific when these items measure different dimensions of ability in a multidimensional test. Multiple speed factors are used to model the speed process by allowing speed to vary across different domains of ability. A joint model for multidimensional abilities and multifactor speed is proposed. Real response time data are analyzed with an exploratory factor analysis as an example to uncover the complex structure of working speed. The feasibility of the proposed model is examined using simulation data. An empirical example with responses and response times is presented to illustrate the proposed model’s applicability and rationality.

Author(s):  
José Luis Párraga Quispe ◽  
Segen F. Estefen ◽  
Nilo de Moura Jorge ◽  
Marcelo Igor Lourenço Souza

During activities of ultra-deepwater exploration using drilling vessels an emergency disconnection between Lower Marine Riser Package (LMRP) and Blowout Preventer (BOP) stack could occur due to extreme environmental conditions. The disconnection is not instantaneous; it takes time due to the discharge of pressurized liquid from the system of hydraulic accumulators and the entire process is known as emergency disconnect sequence — EDS. Therefore, estimate the response time of the BOP control system is important to avoid damages that compromise the drillship safe operation. In this study, the BOP control system uses a hydraulic system constituted of accumulator bottles, a pressure regulator, rams, valves, and connectors. This system is considered to estimate the response time of cutting and sealing the BOP. The response time is estimated by applying the theory of fluid mechanics and Bernoulli equation to calculate the system equilibrium pressure. The accumulators are sized according to API 16 D and considered to have adiabatic behavior. Nitrogen is simulated as real gas. The validation of the proposed model is performed by comparison with a surface test for cutting of drill pipe with blind shear ram. The model is applied to a case study for ultra-deepwater in Campos Basin Brazil.


2019 ◽  
Vol 8 (3) ◽  
pp. 6458-6464

The purpose of this research work is to find out whether one of visual response time or auditory response time is faster than another on web based environment. Simple reaction time can be strong when an individual is asked to press a button as soon as a light or sound appears. A tool for capturing response times is created. This tool is a web application www.responsetime.muhammadsuhaib.com that runs on server and can be accessed through internet. R is commonly used in countless scientific disciplines for statistical analysis. Collected data will be analyzed by using R. it is concluded that on web based environment visual response times are not different than auditory response time. The data are also classified by gender to compare male response time and female response time, data analyzed based on gender, on visual stimuli, male react faster than female. However, on auditory stimuli, there is no different between male and female


2010 ◽  
Vol 5 (3) ◽  
pp. 281-299 ◽  
Author(s):  
Padraic Monaghan ◽  
Morten H. Christiansen ◽  
Thomas A. Farmer ◽  
Stanka A. Fitneva

Phonological Typicality (PT) is a measure of the extent to which a word’s phonology is typical of other words in the lexical category to which it belongs. There is a general coherence among words from the same category in terms of speech sounds, and we have found that words that are phonologically typical of their category tend to be processed more quickly and accurately than words that are less typical. In this paper we describe in greater detail the operationalisation of measures of a word’s PT, and report validations of different parameterisations of the measure. For each variant of PT, we report the extent to which it reflects the coherence of the lexical categories of words in terms of their sound, as well as the extent to which the measure predicts naming and lexical decision response times from a database of monosyllabic word processing. We show that PT is robust to parameter variation, but that measures based on PT of uninflected words (lemmas) best predict response time data for naming and lexical decision of single words.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Annalise Aleta LaPlume

A methodology review paper on the utility and challenges of modelling speed-accuracy trade-offs in response time data. The paper reviews the importance of accounting for speed-accuracy trade-offs when measuring response times, and provides background on diffusion models for response time data. It then describes a practical software implementation of the EZ-diffusion model to model speed-accuracy trade-offs in choice response time data using the R programming language.


2017 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gabriel Tillman

Most current sequential sampling models have random between-trial variability in their parameters. These sources of variability make the models more complex in order to fit response time data, do not provide any further explanation to how the data were generated, and have recently been criticised for allowing infinite flexibility in the models. To explore and test the need of between-trial variability parameters we develop a simple sequential sampling model of N-choice speeded decision making: the racing diffusion model. The model makes speeded decisions from a race of evidence accumulators that integrate information in a noisy fashion within a trial. The racing diffusion does not assume that any evidence accumulation process varies between trial, and so, the model provides alternative explanations of key response time phenomena, such as fast and slow error response times relative to correct response times. Overall, our paper gives good reason to rethink including between-trial variability parameters in sequential sampling models


BJPsych Open ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 7 (S1) ◽  
pp. S223-S223
Author(s):  
Kaj Svedberg

AimsTo improve the one hour response times to referrals made to psychiatric Liaison in A&E without adding or changing available resources.MethodResponse time data of referrals made to the Homerton University Hospital psychiatric liaison service was collected dating back from August 2016 to October 2019 (n = 10225).A nudge was introduced in the form of a large display showing referrals arriving in real time in the staff office.Data was then collected over a period of 5 weeks (n = 436) to measure if any change had occurred in response times.ResultResponse times appear to follow a Poisson like distribution curve. The average referral was responded to within 6 minutes (n = 1577) prior to the nudge, and 6 minutes (n = 88) after. Prior to the nudge the 95% referral envelope fell within 134 minutes (n = 9728) and was 122 minutes (n = 414) after the intervention. Significant statistical difference is observed upon considering response in the first 240 minutes.ConclusionNudge interventions could be a useful resource-sparing method to improve services. The average referral to the HUH liaison team was quickly responded to within 6 minutes and yet hitting the 1 hour 95% target appears ever-elusive. Hitting targets of 95% responses within 1 hour may prove very difficult if we are not considering natural distributions, such as Poisson, occuring in the backgroung which ultimately may require a change in approaches to how we set performance targets.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Yiqin Pan ◽  
Edison M. Choe

Most psychometric models of response times are primarily theory-driven, meaning they are based on various sets of assumptions about how the data should behave. Although useful in certain contexts, such models are often inadequate for the complexities of realistic testing situations and display a poor fit on empirical data. Therefore, as a functional alternative, the present study proposes a data-driven approach, an autoencoder-based response time model, to modeling response times of correctly answered responses. Also, this study introduces the application of the proposed model in anomaly detection (including aberrant examinee and item detection). The result shows this model has an acceptable performance in both response time modeling and anomaly detection.


Author(s):  
Andreas Voss ◽  
Markus Nagler ◽  
Veronika Lerche

Stochastic diffusion models ( Ratcliff, 1978 ) can be used to analyze response time data from binary decision tasks. They provide detailed information about cognitive processes underlying the performance in such tasks. Most importantly, different parameters are estimated from the response time distributions of correct responses and errors that map (1) the speed of information uptake, (2) the amount of information used to make a decision, (3) possible decision biases, and (4) the duration of nondecisional processes. Although this kind of model can be applied to many experimental paradigms and provides much more insight than the analysis of mean response times can, it is still rarely used in cognitive psychology. In the present paper, we provide comprehensive information on the theory of the diffusion model, as well as on practical issues that have to be considered for implementing the model.


Author(s):  
Eugene Gvozdenko ◽  
Dianne Chambers

<span>This paper investigates how monitoring the time spent on a question in a test of basic mathematics skills can provide insights into learning processes, the quality of test takers' knowledge, and cognitive demands and performance of test items that otherwise would remain undiscovered if the usual test outcome of accuracy only format ("correct/incorrect") was used. Data from three tests of basic mathematics skills taken by tertiary students in 2004-2006 were analysed. Means and distributions of individual response times on parallel test questions were examined and differences were further investigated.</span><p>Analysis of response time data revealed a number of surprising findings in regard to the impact of variables on preferences for written and mental calculation methods and regarding additional cognitive demands of a question. The study examined how simple statistical analysis of response time distribution can be used to investigate abnormalities of the item functioning. These findings may be of value to educators and to test producers by informing them about the potential of utilising response time measurements as a diagnostic facility in computerised tests, for the purposes of improving teaching and learning.</p>


2013 ◽  
Vol 155 (A1) ◽  

This paper describes research that was carried-out under the EU FP7 research project SAFEGUARD and presents passenger response time data generated from five full-scale semi-unannounced assembly trials at sea. The data-sets were generated from three different types of passenger ships, a RO-PAX ferry without cabins (RP1), a cruise ship (CS) and a RO-PAX ferry with cabins (RP2). In total, response times from 2366 people were collected making it the largest response time data-set ever collected – on land or sea. The analysis methodology used to extract the response time data and the resultant response time distributions (RTD) is presented. A number of key findings from the data analysis are presented along with three recommendations to modify the IMO guidelines governing ship evacuation analysis, namely; (a) it is inappropriate to use the same RTD for cruise ships and RO-PAX vessels; (b) a new Day Case RTD is suggested for RO-PAX vessels and (c) new Day and Night RTDs are suggested for cruise ships.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document