THE STABILIMENTUM OF THE ORB WEB SPIDER, ARGIOPE ARGENTATA: AN IMPROBABLE DEFENCE AGAINST PREDATORS

1970 ◽  
Vol 102 (6) ◽  
pp. 641-655 ◽  
Author(s):  
Michael H. Robinson ◽  
Barbara Robinson

AbstractThe stabilimentum of orb web spiders is a structure for which several functions, both mechanical and defensive, have been suggested. Argiope argentata (Fabricius) builds a stabilimentum in the form of a white diagonal cross. Analysis of over 2500 webs of this species shows that the perfect cross is seldom built and nearly two thirds of the webs contain no stabilimentum at all. Experiments with wild birds as predators show that they can use stabilimentum-like models in prey location. From these data, and a review of the defensive adaptations of orb web spiders, it is argued that the A. argentata stabilimentum is not an anti-predator device. The evidence for a mechanical function is reviewed and discussed. It is suggested that the stabilimentum provides the spider with a means of making a final adjustment to the mechanical state of the web when this is necessary.

Behaviour ◽  
2011 ◽  
Vol 148 (11-13) ◽  
pp. 1295-1309 ◽  
Author(s):  
André Walter ◽  
Constanze Westphal ◽  
Peter Bliss ◽  
Robin F.A. Moritz

Abstract Water is essential for survival in terrestrial animals. Balancing the water budget can be achieved by avoiding water loss and gaining water. In arthropods drinking as a process of water gain is well investigated in insects. In spiders drinking has only been shown to be present in cursorial spiders but not revealed for web builders. However, some orb web spiders were observed to occasionally ingest water droplets in the web. We here tested whether this reflects drinking. We subjected individual Argiope bruennichi spiders to two different treatments — 'water deprivation' vs. 'water saturation'. We conducted drinking tests by recording the spider's behavioural response to spraying the web with defined amounts of water. After spraying A. bruennichi searched the silk-overstitched web hub for water droplets and ingested them. Individuals that experienced the water deprivation treatment showed significantly more water ingesting behaviours, revealing that this response represents a true drinking mode. All individuals exclusively searched the covered web hubs. We further demonstrated that this structure can retain water for up to 40 min providing an effective substrate for the spiders to drink from. Hence, without the need of leaving the web the silk-covered hubs may help A. bruennichi spiders to balance their water budget.


2001 ◽  
Vol 49 (3) ◽  
pp. 213 ◽  
Author(s):  
F. E. Champion de Crespigny ◽  
M. E. Herberstein ◽  
M. A. Elgar

The foraging behaviour of central-place foragers is thought to be strongly influenced by the distance between the forager and the food source (predator–prey distance). Orb-web spiders are uniquely suited for investigating this idea because they make active foraging decisions towards prey entangled in the web, and they define the dimensions of their foraging arena when they construct the web. Here we manipulate the physiological condition of Argiope keyserlingi and present the spiders with prey of varying quality, in terms of size and accessibility (location within the web and distance from the spider). We found that these spiders adjust their foraging behaviour primarily in response to their physiological condition but, in contrast to other central-place foragers, are indiscriminant of predator–prey distance or the likelihood of escape of the prey. We suggest that these factors are incorporated into the design of the web, and thus increase foraging success through efficient web design.


2011 ◽  
Vol 279 (1734) ◽  
pp. 1824-1830 ◽  
Author(s):  
Shichang Zhang ◽  
Teck Hui Koh ◽  
Wee Khee Seah ◽  
Yee Hing Lai ◽  
Mark A. Elgar ◽  
...  

Spider webs are made of silk, the properties of which ensure remarkable efficiency at capturing prey. However, remaining on, or near, the web exposes the resident spiders to many potential predators, such as ants. Surprisingly, ants are rarely reported foraging on the webs of orb-weaving spiders, despite the formidable capacity of ants to subdue prey and repel enemies, the diversity and abundance of orb-web spiders, and the nutritional value of the web and resident spider. We explain this paradox by reporting a novel property of the silk produced by the orb-web spider Nephila antipodiana (Walckenaer). These spiders deposit on the silk a pyrrolidine alkaloid (2-pyrrolidinone) that provides protection from ant invasion. Furthermore, the ontogenetic change in the production of 2-pyrrolidinone suggests that this compound represents an adaptive response to the threat of natural enemies, rather than a simple by-product of silk synthesis: while 2-pyrrolidinone occurs on the silk threads produced by adult and large juvenile spiders, it is absent on threads produced by small juvenile spiders, whose threads are sufficiently thin to be inaccessible to ants.


2007 ◽  
Vol 55 (1) ◽  
pp. 1 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sean J. Blamires ◽  
Dieter F. Hochuli ◽  
Michael B. Thompson

Antipredator strategies adopted by animals need to compensate for temporal changes. Many orb-web spiders add silk decorations to their webs, which principally attract prey but may attract some predators. To identify their influence on antipredator behaviour in adult female St Andrew’s cross spiders (Argiope keyserlingi) we measured: spider body condition, web characteristics (area, spiral length and decoration building), environmental variables (air temperature, humidity, wind speed, relative prey abundance) and antipredator responses (remaining at the hub, dropping, shifting to the web periphery, or pumping the web) at three distinct periods (July–August, September–October and January–February) in the field. We developed path models from multiple regression analyses to distinguish between factors having direct and indirect effects. We found that even though both antipredator responses and decoration building change over time, antipredator responses and decoration investment are independent. Body condition and wind speed are directly positively associated with pumping frequency, and decoration building is negatively associated with the frequency of remaining at the hub because the likelihood that an object approaching the web is a predator increases if decorations are added. Wind speed is positively associated with antipredator behaviour and decoration building, due to an increased rate of feeding affecting body condition.


Behaviour ◽  
2008 ◽  
Vol 145 (3) ◽  
pp. 277-295 ◽  
Author(s):  
Felipe Gawryszewski ◽  
Paulo Motta

AbstractSeveral orb-web spiders build conspicuous decorations in their webs. The prey attraction hypothesis proposes that decorations increase spider foraging success by attracting prey, and that attraction is linked to UV reflectance. Alternatively, the web advertisement hypothesis proposes that decorations are a signal that advertises the presence of the web to large animals. We tested both hypotheses for the web silk tufts of Gasteracantha cancriformis. Even though tufts are UV reflective, we did not find support for the prey attraction hypothesis. In the field, when webs with tufts painted black and control webs were compared, there were no differences in the number of prey captured, number of damaged areas in webs and type of prey captured. In the laboratory, Drosophila melanogaster did not demonstrate preference for tufted silk lines versus non-tufted silk lines. Our data also did not give support for the web advertisement hypothesis. The proportion of web destruction was similar between web with tufts painted black and control webs during four days of experimentation. Therefore, two of the most favoured hypotheses that attempt to explain decorations do not apply for web silk tufts in our study system. Instead we propose that silk tufts might be an aposematic signal.


2007 ◽  
Vol 55 (1) ◽  
pp. 9 ◽  
Author(s):  
Dinesh Rao ◽  
Ken Cheng ◽  
Marie E. Herberstein

A long-running debate in the spider literature concerns the function of the extra silk decorations in some spider webs. These decorations are appended to the web and constitute a highly visible signal, which is inconsistent with the trend towards web invisibility. Despite the sustained attention of researchers, the exact function of these decorations is yet to be understood. While most studies have focussed on testing particular hypotheses, there has been a dearth of natural history data regarding web decorations in field conditions. In this study we present baseline data regarding the influence of seasonality, microhabitat characteristics and ecology on the presence of web decorations in an Australian orb web spider, Argiope keyserlingi. In particular, we show that there is preference among spiders to build their webs between bushes and to face the south-east, but this preference does not influence decoration building.


2000 ◽  
Vol 48 (2) ◽  
pp. 217 ◽  
Author(s):  
M. E. Herberstein

Orb web spiders in the genus Argiope attach highly visible silk bands called decorations or stabilimenta to their webs. Two different hypotheses regarding the function of these structures were investigated in the field using Argiope keyserlingi: prey attraction and anti-predatory device. The first hypothesis suggests that web decorations attract prey to the web, and webs carrying decorations will capture more prey than those without. A field census of prey capture showed that webs adorned with more decorative bands indeed captured more but similarly sized prey per hour compared with webs carrying fewer decorations. Web height or web size, however, were not related to the rate of prey capture. This pattern was confirmed by a paired comparison of prey-capture rates within individuals that increased or decreased the number of decorative bands on consecutive days. Individuals that used more decorations also captured more prey compared with days when they spun fewer decorations. The second hypothesis suggests that these structures function as anti-predatory devices and, consequently, spiders on decorated webs benefit from a lower rate of mortality than spiders on undecorated webs. A census of the mortality rates of spiders over 19 days revealed that spiders did not disappear from undecorated webs more frequently than from decorated webs. Consequently, the idea that web decorations act as anti-predatory devices in A. keyserlingi was not supported.


2013 ◽  
Vol 9 (4) ◽  
pp. 20130052 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kensuke Nakata

Although it is well known that spatial learning can be important in the biology of predators that actively move around in search for food, comparatively little is known about ways in which spatial learning might function in the strategies of sit-and-wait predators. In this study, Cyclosa octotuberculata , an orb-web spider that uses its legs to contract radial threads of its web to increase thread tension, was trained to capture prey in limited web sectors. After training, spiders that had captured prey in horizontal web sectors applied more tension on radial threads connected to horizontal sectors than spiders that had captured prey in vertical sectors. This result suggests that the effect of experience on C. octotuberculata 's behaviour is not expressed in the way the trained spider responds to prey-derived stimuli and is instead expressed in behaviour by which the spider anticipates the likely direction from which prey will arrive in the future. This illustrates that learning can be important even when the predator remains in one location during foraging bouts.


2021 ◽  
pp. 107754632199354
Author(s):  
Ramón Zaera ◽  
Óscar Serrano ◽  
José Fernández-Sáez ◽  
Antonino Morassi

Orb web spiders locate the position of a perturbation in the web because of a prey impact through highly sensitive slit sensilla at the tip of their legs. Thus, the web serves as a self-made extension of its sensory space which transmits vibrations from the perturbation point to the spider location. These vibrations may contain the information required by the spider to rapidly identify the position where the prey has impacted and approach to it before it flies away. For axially symmetric orb webs supported at the boundary and for a spider which stays at the center of the web, it was shown that the knowledge of the transverse deflection time history at the eight spider legs, for a sufficiently large interval of time, contains enough information to localize the position of the prey. In this article, we address the same inverse problem of localization of the prey, but we suppose that the spider knows only a small number of selected information of the transverse displacement, such as, for example, the maxima of the response of the time history at the eight control points. We show how this reduced information still allows the spider to obtain a fairly accurate angular localization of the prey, for different prey and orb web characteristics.


2010 ◽  
Vol 6 (5) ◽  
pp. 585-588 ◽  
Author(s):  
Klaas W. Welke ◽  
Jutta M. Schneider

Costs of inbreeding can lead to total reproductive failure and inbreeding avoidance is, therefore, common. In classical sex roles with no paternal care, the selective pressure to avoid inbreeding is mostly on the female, which carries the higher costs. In some orb-web spiders, this situation is very different because females are polyandrous and males are monogynous or at most bigynous. Additionally, females of many entelegyne orb weavers are thought to bias paternity post-copulatorily towards a desired mate. This increases the selective pressure on males to adjust their investment in a mating with regard to the compatibility to a female. Here, we examine whether genetic relatedness influences mating behaviour in the orb-web spider Argiope bruennichi . We mated either a sibling or a non-sibling male to a female in single copulation trials and compared copulation duration, cannibalism rate and female fecundity. Our experiment revealed that males prolonged their copulation duration and were cannibalized more frequently when mating with a non-sibling female. Males mating with a sibling female were more likely to escape cannibalism by copulating briefly, thus presumably increasing their chances of re-mating with a more compatible female. This suggests that males can adaptively adjust their investment relating to the compatibility of a female.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document