probe array
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

274
(FIVE YEARS 26)

H-INDEX

24
(FIVE YEARS 1)

2022 ◽  
Vol 175 ◽  
pp. 113011
Author(s):  
L.Y. Meng ◽  
J.C. Xu ◽  
J.B. Liu ◽  
L. Cao ◽  
P. Wang ◽  
...  
Keyword(s):  

2021 ◽  
Vol 16 (0) ◽  
pp. 1202078-1202078
Author(s):  
Ryo SOMEYA ◽  
Haruaki TANAKA ◽  
Yugo FUNATO ◽  
Yunhan CAI ◽  
Moe AKIMITSU ◽  
...  

2021 ◽  
Vol 92 (5) ◽  
pp. 053518
Author(s):  
Zhengbo Cheng ◽  
Yi Tan ◽  
Zhe Gao ◽  
Shouzhi Wang ◽  
Binbin Wang ◽  
...  

2021 ◽  
Vol 92 (5) ◽  
pp. 053545
Author(s):  
K. Akashi ◽  
Y. Iijima ◽  
D. Kobayashi ◽  
T. Asai ◽  
T. Roche ◽  
...  

2021 ◽  
Vol 92 (5) ◽  
pp. 053523
Author(s):  
J. G. Watkins ◽  
H. Q. Wang ◽  
D. Thomas ◽  
C. Murphy ◽  
D. Taussig ◽  
...  

Author(s):  
Pushpendra Singh ◽  
Komal Saxena ◽  
Pathik Sahoo ◽  
Subrata Ghosh ◽  
Anirban Bandyopadhyay

Since the 1960s, it is held that when a neuron fires, a nerve spike passes only through the selective branches, the calculated choice is a key to learning by rewiring. It is argued by chemically estimating the membrane's ion channel density that different axonal branches get active to pass the spike -branches blink at firing at different time domains. Here, using a new time-lapse dielectric imaging, we visualize the classic branch selection process, hidden circuits operating at different time domains become visible. The fractal grid of coaxial probes captures wireless snapshots of material's vibration at various depths below the membrane by setting a suitable frequency. Thus far, branch selection observed emitted energy or particle but never the emitters, what they do. Since each dielectric material transmits & reflects signals of different frequencies, we image live how filaments search for many branch-made-circuits, choose an unique pathway 103 times faster than a single nerve spike. It reveals that neural branches and circuit visible in a microscope is not absolute, there coexist many circuits each operating in different dime domains, operating at a time.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ilenia Paparella ◽  
Liuba Papeo

Working memory (WM) uses knowledge and relations to organize and store multiple individual items in a smaller set of structured units, or chunks. We investigated whether a crowd of individuals that exceeds the WM is retained and, therefore, recognized more accurately, if individuals are represented as interacting with one another –i.e., they form social chunks. Further, we asked what counts as a social chunk in WM: two individuals involved in a meaningful interaction or just spatially close and face-to-face. In three experiments with a delayed change-detection task, participants had to report whether a probe-array was the same of, or different from a sample-array featuring two or three dyads of bodies either face-to-face (facing array) or back-to-back (non-facing array). In Experiment 1, where facing dyads depicted coherent, meaningful interactions, participants were more accurate to detect changes in facing (vs. non-facing) arrays. A similar advantage was found in Experiment 2, even though facing dyads depicted no meaningful interaction. In Experiment 3, we introduced a secondary task (verbal shadowing) to increase WM load. This manipulation abolished the advantage of facing (vs. non-facing) arrays, only when facing dyads depicted no meaningful interactions. These results show that WM uses representation of interaction to chunk crowds in social groups. The mere facingness of bodies is sufficient on its own to evoke representation of interaction, thus defining a social chunk in WM; although the lack of semantic anchor makes chunking fainter and more susceptible to interference of a secondary task.


2020 ◽  
Vol 91 (12) ◽  
pp. 123702
Author(s):  
Fengming Sun ◽  
Zhenyu Zhu ◽  
Long Ma

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document