scholarly journals Cognitive performance in Early Head Start interventions among infants 0-3 years: The impact of early childhood risk factors

2015 ◽  
Vol 81 (1) ◽  
pp. 207
Author(s):  
S. Kumar
2002 ◽  
Vol 59 (3) ◽  
pp. 215 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sara R. Jaffee ◽  
Terrie E. Moffitt ◽  
Avshalom Caspi ◽  
Eric Fombonne ◽  
Richie Poulton ◽  
...  

Author(s):  
Andrew Seltzer

The Children’s Aid Society (CAS) early childhood initiative is located in two of our New York City community schools, Primary School (PS) 5 and PS 8, in the Washington Heights section of northern Manhattan. This initiative was conceived as a partnership between the New York City Board of Education and CAS. The collaboration brought newborns and their families into the schools in which the children would complete fifth grade. The initiative began in 1994 and has been in full operation since 1996. Since then, the need for such a project has been confirmed and experience has provided insights into how a program for pregnant women and children through age five (often called a Zero to Five Program) can be effectively implemented within a public school. The CAS Zero to Five model connects two federally funded programs—Early Head Start (birth to age three) and Head Start (ages three to five)—to provide comprehensive educational and social services to low-income families and their children. The population attending the Zero to Five Program confronts the obstacles facing all new immigrant families living in poverty in an urban setting. In both schools more than 75% of the families are from the Dominican Republic; another 20% come from other Central and South American countries. The parents’ language is Spanish, and language barriers and acculturation issues result in social isolation. In addition, because many residents lack legal documentation, they are reluctant to access health and social services. The few early childhood programs in the neighborhood all have long waiting lists. A majority of the families share overcrowded apartments with other families or extended family; whole families often live in one bedroom where books and age-appropriate toys are scarce and there may be little child-centered language interaction. However, in spite of the difficulties, these parents have a drive to succeed and they understand the importance of education. By combining and linking Early Head Start and Head Start programs and integrating them into a community school, the CAS Zero to Five Program provides children and families with quality educational, health, and social services, after which the children transition into public school classes within the same building.


2008 ◽  
Vol 33 (7) ◽  
pp. 739-750 ◽  
Author(s):  
C. Joinson ◽  
J. Heron ◽  
A. von Gontard ◽  
U. Butler ◽  
J. Golding ◽  
...  

2011 ◽  
Vol 14 (6) ◽  
pp. 544-552 ◽  
Author(s):  
Venla S. Laitala ◽  
Jacob Hjelmborg ◽  
Markku Koskenvuo ◽  
Ismo Räihä ◽  
Juha O. Rinne ◽  
...  

We analyzed the association between mean height and old age cognition in two Nordic twin cohorts with different childhood living conditions. The cognitive performance of 4720 twin individuals from Denmark (mean age 81.6 years, SD = 4.59) and Finland (mean age 74.4 years, SD = 5.26) was measured using validated cognitive screens. Taller height was associated with better cognitive performance in Finland (β-estimates 0.18 SD/10cm, p value < .001, for men and 0.13 SD, p = .008, for women), but this association was not significant in Denmark (β-estimates 0.0093 SD, p value = .16, for men and 0.0075 SD, p value = .016, for women) when adjusted for age and education/social class. Among Finnish participants higher variability of cognitive performance within shorter height quintiles was observed. Analysis using gene-environment interaction models showed that environmental factors exerted a greater impact on cognitive performance in shorter participants, whereas in taller participants' it was explained mainly by genetic factors. Our results suggest that shorter participants with childhood adversity are more vulnerable to environmental risk factors for cognitive impairment.


2009 ◽  
Vol 20 (6) ◽  
pp. 883-892 ◽  
Author(s):  
Robert H. Bradley ◽  
Rachel Chazan-Cohen ◽  
Helen Raikes

2019 ◽  
Vol 53 (1) ◽  
pp. 26-46
Author(s):  
Shelley Stagg Peterson ◽  
Soon Young Jang ◽  
Jayson San Miguel ◽  
Sandra Styres ◽  
Audrey Madsen

Five Aboriginal Head Start early childhood educators from a northern Canadian community participated in interviews for the purpose of informing non-Indigenous teachers’ classroom teaching. Their observations and experiences highlight the importance of learning from and on the land alongside family members, and of family stability and showing acceptance of all children. Additionally, participants talked of the impact of residential schools on their families in terms of loss of their Indigenous language, and their attempts to learn and to teach the children in their classrooms the Indigenous languages and teachings.


2018 ◽  
Vol 90 (6) ◽  
pp. 358-367 ◽  
Author(s):  
Éadaoin M. Butler ◽  
José G.B. Derraik ◽  
Rachael W. Taylor ◽  
Wayne S. Cutfield

Statistical models have been developed for the prediction or diagnosis of a wide range of outcomes. However, to our knowledge, only 7 published studies have reported models to specifically predict overweight and/or obesity in early childhood. These models were developed using known risk factors and vary greatly in terms of their discrimination and predictive capacities. There are currently no established guidelines on what constitutes an acceptable level of risk (i.e., risk threshold) for childhood obesity prediction models, but these should be set following consideration of the consequences of false-positive and false-negative predictions, as well as any relevant clinical guidelines. To date, no studies have examined the impact of using early childhood obesity prediction models as intervention tools. While these are potentially valuable to inform targeted interventions, the heterogeneity of the existing models and the lack of consensus on adequate thresholds limit their usefulness in practice.


2017 ◽  
Vol 17 (4) ◽  
pp. 411-415 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sarah E. Domoff ◽  
Julie C. Lumeng ◽  
Niko Kaciroti ◽  
Alison L. Miller

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document