scholarly journals TO003CROSSTALK OF UREMIC TOXINS AND INFLAMMASOME IN CHRONIC KIDNEY DISEASE-RELATED COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT

2020 ◽  
Vol 35 (Supplement_3) ◽  
Author(s):  
LUNG-CHIH LI ◽  
WEI-YU CHEN ◽  
Jin Po Chen ◽  
Wen-Chin Lee ◽  
JENG-LIN YANG

Abstract Background and Aims Chronic kidney disease (CKD) is getting prevalent and causes big burden to the health care systems worldwide. CKD-associated cognitive impairment (CI) not only influences the behave function, but also results in longer hospitalization and higher risk of mortality. Inflammation is an important pathogenesis of CKD and plays a crucial role in progression of CKD. In this study, we hypothesized that accumulated uremic toxins, especially indoxyl sulfate (IS) and P-cresyl sulfate (PCS) induce neuroinflammation via activation of NLRP3 inflammasome in the brain tissue of CKD animals. This study aims to delineate the crosstalk between uremic toxins and inflammation in CKD-induced CI. Method Eight-week-old male C57B6 wildtype and NLRP3 knockout mice received sham and 5/6 nephrectomy to mimic CKD status. AST-120 (Kremezin), a spherical carbon adsorbent, was given orally to mice to neutralize the accumulated uremic toxins. The Y-maze and Morris water maze (MWM) testes were applied to evaluate cognitive function, including the spatial, short-term and long-term memory in shame, CKD, and AST-120 treated CKD mice. HPLC was used to examine the concentration of IS and PCS in the serum and brain tissues. Western blot and immunohistochemistry stain were used to identify the protein expression of target molecules in the brain tissue. Results CKD mice showed impaired spatial, short-term and long-term memory; however, 10% AST-120 attenuated CKD-induced memory impairment (Figure 1). IS, but not PCS, elevated in the brain tissues, especially in the frontal cortex and hippocampus. AST-120 reduced CKD-induced IS elevation in the brain tissue, and also ameliorated NLRP3 inflammasome activation in the astrocytes in the frontal and hippocampus. Finally, NLRP3 deficiency reversed the CKD-induced memory impairment (Figure 2). Conclusion IS is the major uremic toxin that causes CI in CKD mice via activating NLRP3 inflammasome pathway in the brain tissue. However, AST-120 effectively ameliorates CKD-induced CI.

SIMULATION ◽  
1964 ◽  
Vol 3 (6) ◽  
pp. 46-51
Author(s):  
W. Ross Adey ◽  
N.V. Findler

It is attempted in this paper to give a mathematical description of the short-term memory. Instead of using the microscopic properties of individual neu rons, such as the number of interconnections, neuron density, threshold value, etc., the cerebral cortex is regarded as a "neuron gas" that is a vast conglomer ate of neurons with statistically distributed charac teristics. Stimuli from the environment cause the receptor neurons to emit virtual electromagnetic waves into the brain. A self-optimizing process of the brain tis sue is here described by which the useful, informa tion-carrying energy reaching the long-term memory tends to maximum. It is emphasized that in the following a brain model is described and the physical processes in volved may have no direct equivalent in reality.


PLoS ONE ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 16 (4) ◽  
pp. e0250079
Author(s):  
Fernanda Ferreira Evangelista ◽  
Willian Costa-Ferreira ◽  
Francini Martini Mantelo ◽  
Lucimara Fátima Beletini ◽  
Amanda Hinobu de Souza ◽  
...  

The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of rosuvastatin treatment on memory impairment, and anxiogenic-like effects in mice chronically infected with Toxoplasma gondii. For this, Balb/c mice were infected orally with chronic ME-49 strain of Toxoplasma gondii. Oral treatment with rosuvastatin (40mg/kg/day) started on the 51st day post-infection and was performed daily for 21 days. After completion of treatment, anxiety-like effects and locomotion were investigated in the open field (OF) test, whereas novel object recognition (NOR) test was used for evaluation of short- and long-term memory. At the end of the experiments, the brain was collected for Toxoplasma gondii DNA quantification and histopathological analysis. Infection with ME-49 strain decreased the time spent in the center of OF, indicating an anxiogenic effect, without affecting total and peripheral locomotion. Rosuvastatin treatment inhibited the change in the center time. Besides, pharmacological treatment increased total and central locomotion in both non-infected and infected animals. Infection also impaired both short- and long-term memory in the NOR test, and these effects were reverted by rosuvastatin treatment. In addition to effects in behavioral changes, rosuvastatin also reduced parasite load in the brain and attenuated signs of brain inflammation such as perivascular cuffs, inflammatory cell infiltration and tissue damage. These findings indicate for the first time the efficacy of rosuvastatin in treatment of memory impairment and anxiogenic effect evoked by infection with Toxoplasma gondii. These effects might be mediated by reduced cyst load, which in turn decrease inflammation and damage in the brain.


2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
cika suteja

In daily life, humans can not be separated from the name of learning and remembering. Learning and remembering is the basis for someone to adapt their behavior to the environment. The brain is the organ that regulates learning and memory. The brain will store and bring back the information needed. Decline in memory usually occurs during old age but does not rule out the possibility of happening at a young age too. Decreasing memory recall at a young age is usually caused by a lack of physical activity. Sports is one of the factors that can improve memory. Exercise can improve blood circulation so that the brain can be well-nourished and will work optimally. To make our memories into long-term memories can be done by repeating the information obtained. Because repetition can accelerate and increase consolidation. Information entered in long-term memory will be grouped according to the type of information. This grouping serves to facilitate the search for the desired information. Short-term information will forever be forgotten, but long-term information can last up to a lifetime.


Encyclopedia ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 2 (1) ◽  
pp. 26-35
Author(s):  
Natalia A. Shnayder ◽  
Timur K. Sirbiladze ◽  
Irina V. Demko ◽  
Marina M. Petrova ◽  
Regina F. Nasyrova

Limbic encephalitis (LE) is an inflammatory disease of the brain, in which lesion is anatomically limited in structures of the limbic system. In some cases, LE can start with symptoms of limbic dysfunction with further involvement of other regions of the brain. Classic LE syndrome includes such symptoms as the development of personality disorders, depression, sleep disorders, epileptic seizures, hallucinations and cognitive disorders (short-term and long-term memory impairment). The information of clinical examination, electroencephalogram (EEG), magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and cerebrospinal fluid studies (CSF) suggest the diagnosis of LE in most patients with Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19).


2018 ◽  
Vol 155 ◽  
pp. 01026
Author(s):  
Vladimir Shumilov ◽  
Vladimir Syryamkin

In this paper, various types of memory are considered.Development of the nervous system and the brain of organisms ensures expansion of species with more developed brain. Different characteristics of the brain zones have created different conditions for forming traces of events. The various traces of trace events formed in different parts of the brain provided a wider range of functions and brain responses, which contributed to the survival and expansion of species. The work describes the appearance of memory varieties and the causes of these varieties.


2001 ◽  
Vol 88 (1) ◽  
pp. 102-110 ◽  
Author(s):  
L. N. Vaillancourt ◽  
M. A. Persinger

The concept of the neuromatrix assumes that all behaviours are associated with complex spatiotemporal electromagnetic fields within the brain. The same complex magnetic field can be supported potentially by different mosaics of neuronal associations and result in the same behaviours. In the present study the accuracy of long-term and short-term spatial memory for rats that had been treated with 100 mg/kg of ketamine immediately after the induction of seizures by lithium and pilocarpine did not differ from normal rats despite the conspicuous multifocal neuronal loss. A 30-min. delay of treatment with ketamine resulted in significant memory impairment. Whereas deficits in short-term memory were significantly correlated with cell loss within the CA field of the hippocampus, deficits in long-term memory were related to cell loss within specific thalamic-amygdaloid structures. Implications for the concept of the neuromatrix, if valid, are discussed.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ni-Chun Chung ◽  
Aeson Chang ◽  
Ryan Gillis ◽  
Erica Sloan ◽  
Adam K Walker

Abstract BackgroundCancer-related cognitive impairment (CRCI) has been reported in cancer survivors 20 years or more after cancer treatment, and has been associated with sustained increases in circulating inflammatory biomarkers. One of the major risk factors for CRCI is chemotherapy, and preclinical studies typically examine the impact of chemotherapy in cancer naïve mice to evaluate potential mechanisms However, clinical evaluation of the long-term effects of chemotherapy cannot avoid the potential cumulative impact of preceding factors on the brain including the cancer itself and cancer surgery. MethodsTo evaluate the cumulative impact of cancer-related factors on cognitive impairment and hippocampal cytokine expression, we evaluated the effect of paclitaxel chemotherapy vs. placebo on a background of 67NR mammary carcinoma and surgical resection of the primary tumour in mice. Memory was assessed using the Y maze test and novel object/novel place recognition test. Changes in hippocampal pro-inflammatory and anti-inflammatory cytokines, microglia and neuron markers were assessed using qRT-PCR. Results Cancer and cancer surgery was sufficient to induce long-term memory impairment and sustained increases in hippocampal pro-inflammatory cytokines. Paclitaxel prolonged spatial memory impairment in the Y maze test and exacerbated hippocampal Il6 and Tnfa mRNA expression compared with placebo treatment. ConclusionsThese findings suggest that cancer and cancer surgery can sensitise the brain to an exaggerated neuroinflammatory response to chemotherapy, and may contribute to sustained chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment observed in cancer survivors.


Blood ◽  
2012 ◽  
Vol 120 (21) ◽  
pp. 664-664
Author(s):  
Gregory T. Armstrong ◽  
Petersen C. Ronald ◽  
Nan Zhang ◽  
Aimee Santucci ◽  
Deokumar Srivastava ◽  
...  

Abstract Abstract 664 Background: Survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia (ALL) who received prophylactic cranial radiation therapy (CRT) are at increased risk for deficits in neurocognitive skills, including attention, working memory and processing speed. As survivors age, global brain injury from CRT may reduce cognitive reserve, placing them at risk for early onset dementia or long-term memory deficits. The prevalence of dementia and memory abnormalities in adult survivors of childhood ALL has not previously been established. Methods: Analyses were conducted on 265 of 445 (60%) eligible survivors of childhood ALL (median age 36 years, range 26–54 years; 52% female) treated with 18Gy (n=127) or 24Gy (n=138) CRT with a median time from CRT of 30 years (range 15–46 years). Participants completed the Wechsler Memory Scale IV, including the Brief Cognitive Status Exam (BSCE), and the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). Age-adjusted standard scores were calculated and the BSCE was also adjusted for education level. Prevalence of memory impairment (<1 SD below age-expected mean), stratified by RT dose exposure, is reported and logistic regression used to identify risk factors for impairment. A subset of survivors (n=85) completed brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), including assessment of hippocampal volume, cortical thickness, white matter volume, diffusion tensor imaging, and functional MRI (fMRI) during a cued-recall memory task. Results: Survivors who received 24 Gy CRT had increased impairment on global measures of both short-term recall (33%; p<0.001) and long-term recall (30%; p<0.001), while no increase in impairment was seen after 18Gy. Impaired short-term recall was associated with smaller right (p=0.02) and left (p<0.01) temporal lobe volumes, while impaired long-term recall was associated with thinner parietal and frontal cortices. On subtests evaluating narrative memory (i.e. story recall) and design memory, increasing RT dose (24 vs. 18Gy) was associated with an increased prevalence of long-term memory impairment (narrative: 28% vs. 12%, p=0.001; designs: 13% vs. 3.2%, p=0.003). However, no CRT dose response was identified for short term narrative and design memory. Survivors with impaired long-term memory for designs demonstrated a compensatory increase in left hippocampal fMRI activation (p=0.005), and the effect was greater in the higher dose group (p = 0.04). The mean score for long-term narrative memory among survivors who received 24Gy was equivalent to the mean score of a 70–74 year old adult population. Neither young age (0–4 years) at CRT (Odds Ratio [OR] 1.4, 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.8–2.7), time from CRT (OR 1.0, 95% CI 0.9–1.1) nor intrathecal methotrexate exposure (OR 3.9, 95% CI 0.4–36.1) were significantly associated with long-term memory deficits. Reduced cognitive status (by BSCE) was identified after 18Gy (9%, p=0.11) and 24Gy (18%, p<0.001), suggesting a CRT dose-response effect. On diffusion tensor imaging, increased radial diffusivity in the frontal, parietal and temporal regions, an inverse measure of white matter integrity, was associated with reduced BSCE. Current employment rates were equivalent (63%) in both CRT dose groups, suggesting no difference in functional status. Conclusions: Aging adult survivors of ALL who received 24Gy CRT have reduced cognitive status and significant impairment in short-term and long-term memory. There appears to be a dose response effect selective for long-term narrative and design memory, but not for short term narrative and design memory. These patterns are consistent with early onset of age-related (long-term) memory loss, and early stage dementia, yet at a median age of only 36 years. After 24Gy, survivors have the narrative memory equivalent to a 70 year-old in the general population. Survivors with memory impairment demonstrated reduced integrity on structural and functional neuroimaging in anatomical regions established as essential for memory formation and long-term recall. However, these memory impairments do not seem to affect functional status (employment rates) suggesting that, rather than frank dementia, deficits in middle adulthood are consistent with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Longitudinal evaluation of this population is needed as MCI often progresses into early onset dementia with age. Disclosures: No relevant conflicts of interest to declare.


2003 ◽  
Vol 26 (6) ◽  
pp. 743-744
Author(s):  
Barbara J. Knowlton ◽  
Indre V. Viskontas

Studies of neuropsychological patients are relevant to models of how long-term memories are stored. If amnesia is considered a binding deficit and not a difficulty in transferring information from short-term to long-term memory, it is unclear why context-free semantic learning is impaired. Also the model should account for the reverse temporal gradient seen in patients with semantic dementia.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document