filtered noise
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

91
(FIVE YEARS 12)

H-INDEX

18
(FIVE YEARS 1)

Author(s):  
Laura Torres Borda ◽  
Yannick Jadoul ◽  
Heikki Rasilo ◽  
Anna Salazar Casals ◽  
Andrea Ravignani

Vocal plasticity can occur in response to environmental and biological factors, including conspecifics' vocalizations and noise. Pinnipeds are one of the few mammalian groups capable of vocal learning, and are therefore relevant to understanding the evolution of vocal plasticity in humans and other animals. Here, we investigate the vocal plasticity of harbour seals ( Phoca vitulina ), a species with vocal learning abilities observed in adulthood but not puppyhood. To evaluate early mammalian vocal development, we tested 1–3 weeks-old seal pups. We tailored noise playbacks to this species and age to induce seal pups to shift their fundamental frequency ( f 0 ), rather than adapt call amplitude or temporal characteristics. We exposed individual pups to low- and high-intensity bandpass-filtered noise, which spanned—and masked—their typical range of f 0 ; simultaneously, we recorded pups' spontaneous calls. Unlike most mammals, pups modified their vocalizations by lowering their f 0 in response to increased noise. This modulation was precise and adapted to the particular experimental manipulation of the noise condition. In addition, higher levels of noise induced less dispersion around the mean f 0 , suggesting that pups may have actively focused their phonatory efforts to target lower frequencies. Noise did not seem to affect call amplitude. However, one seal showed two characteristics of the Lombard effect known for human speech in noise: significant increase in call amplitude and flattening of spectral tilt. Our relatively low noise levels may have favoured f 0 modulation while inhibiting amplitude adjustments. This lowering of f 0 is unusual, as most animals commonly display no such f 0 shift. Our data represent a relatively rare case in mammalian neonates, and have implications for the evolution of vocal plasticity and vocal learning across species, including humans. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Voice modulation: from origin and mechanism to social impact (Part I)’.


Author(s):  
Michaela Klimova ◽  
Ilona M Bloem ◽  
Sam Ling

Normalization within visual cortex is modulated by contextual influences; stimuli sharing similar features suppress each other more than dissimilar stimuli. This feature-tuned component of suppression depends on multiple factors, including the orientation content of stimuli. Indeed, pairs of stimuli arranged in a center-surround configuration attenuate each other's response to a greater degree when oriented collinearly than when oriented orthogonally. While numerous studies have examined the nature of surround suppression at these two extremes, far less is known about how the strength of tuned normalization varies as a function of continuous changes in orientation similarity, particularly in humans. In this study, we used fMRI to examine the bandwidth of orientation-tuned suppression within human visual cortex. BOLD responses were acquired as participants viewed a full-field circular stimulus composed of wedges of orientation-bandpass filtered noise. This stimulus configuration allowed us to parametrically vary orientation differences between neighboring wedges in gradual steps between collinear and orthogonal. We found the greatest suppression for collinearly arranged stimuli, with a gradual increase in BOLD response as the orientation content became more dissimilar. We quantified the tuning width of orientation-tuned suppression, finding that the voxelwise bandwidth of orientation tuned normalization was between 20-30 degrees, and did not differ substantially between V1-V3. Voxelwise analyses revealed that suppression width covaried with retinotopic preference, with the tightest bandwidths at outer eccentricities. Having an estimate of orientation-tuned suppression bandwidth can serve to constrain models of tuned normalization, establishing the precise degree to which suppression strength depends on similarity between visual stimulus components.


2021 ◽  
Vol 131 ◽  
pp. 103689
Author(s):  
Saeed Gheisari Hasnijeh ◽  
Arvid Naess ◽  
Mehrdad Poursina ◽  
Hossein Karimpour

2021 ◽  
Vol 12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Bavo Van Kerrebroeck ◽  
Pieter-Jan Maes

Since sound and music are powerful forces and drivers of human behavior and physiology, we propose the use of sonification to activate healthy breathing patterns in participants to induce relaxation. Sonification is often used in the context of biofeedback as it can represent an informational, non-invasive and real-time stimulus to monitor, motivate or modify human behavior. The first goal of this study is the proposal and evaluation of a distance-based biofeedback system using a tempo- and phase-aligned sonification strategy to adapt breathing patterns and induce states of relaxation. A second goal is the evaluation of several sonification stimuli on 18 participants that were recruited online and of which we analyzed psychometric and behavioral data using, respectively questionnaires and respiration rate and ratio. Sonification stimuli consisted of filtered noise mimicking a breathing sound, nature environmental sounds and a musical phrase. Preliminary results indicated the nature stimulus as most pleasant and as leading to the most prominent decrease of respiration rate. The noise sonification had the most beneficial effect on respiration ratio. While further research is needed to generalize these findings, this study and its methodological underpinnings suggest the potential of the proposed biofeedback system to perform ecologically valid experiments at participants' homes during the COVID-19 pandemic.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Laura Torres Borda ◽  
Yannick Jadoul ◽  
Heikki Rasilo ◽  
Anna Salazar Casals ◽  
Andrea Ravignani

ABSTRACTVocal plasticity can occur in response to environmental and biological factors, including conspecifics’ vocalisations and noise. Pinnipeds are one of the few mammalian groups capable of vocal learning, and are therefore relevant to understanding the evolution of vocal plasticity in humans and other animals. Here, we investigate the vocal plasticity of harbour seals (Phoca vitulina), a species with vocal learning abilities attested in adulthood but not puppyhood. To zoom into early mammalian vocal development, we tested 1-3 weeks old seal pups. We tailored noise playbacks to this species and age to induce seal pups to shift their fundamental frequency (F0), rather than adapt call amplitude or temporal characteristics. We exposed individual pups to bandpass-filtered noise, which purposely spanned – and masked – their typical range of F0s, and simultaneously recorded pups’ spontaneous calls. Seals were able to modify their vocalisations quite unlike most mammals: They lowered their F0 in response to increased noise. This modulation was punctual and adapted to the particular noise condition. In addition, higher noise induced less dispersion around the mean F0, suggesting that pups may have been actively focusing their phonatory efforts to target lower frequencies. Noise masking did not seem to affect call amplitude. However, one seal showed two characteristics of the Lombard effect known for human speech in noise: significant increase in call amplitude and flattening of spectral tilt. Our relatively low noise levels may have favoured F0 shifts while inhibiting amplitude adjustments. This lowering of F0 is quite unusual, as other animals commonly display no F0 shift independently of noise amplitude. Our data represents a relatively rare case in mammalian neonates, and may have implications for the evolution of vocal plasticity across species, including humans.


2020 ◽  
Vol 10 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Soheil M. Doustkouhi ◽  
Philip R. K. Turnbull ◽  
Steven C. Dakin

AbstractSubjective refraction is the gold-standard for prescribing refractive correction, but its accuracy is limited by patient’s subjective judgment about their clarity of vision. We asked if an involuntary eye movement, optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), could serve as an objective measure of visual-clarity, specifically measuring the dependence of OKN—elicited by drifting spatial-frequency filtered noise—on mean spherical equivalent (MSE) refractive error. In Experiment 1 we quantified OKN score—a measure of consistency with stimulus-direction—for participants with different MSEs. Estimates of MSE based on OKN scores correlate well with estimates of MSE made using autorefraction (r = 0.878, p < 0.001, Bland–Altman analysis: mean difference of 0.00D (95% limits of agreement: − 0.85 to + 0.85D). In Experiment 2, we quantified the relationship between OKN gain (ratio of tracking eye-movement velocity to stimulus velocity) and MSEs (− 2.00, − 1.00, − 0.50, 0.00 and + 1.00D) induced with contact lenses for each participant. The mean difference between measures of MSE based on autorefraction or on OKN gain was + 0.05D (− 0.90 to + 1.01D), and the correlation of these measures across participants was r = 0.976, p < 0.001. Results indicate that MSE attenuates OKN gain so that OKN can be used as an objective proxy for patient response to select the best corrective lens.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lina Motlagh Zadeh ◽  
Noah Silbert ◽  
De Wet Swanepoel ◽  
David R Moore

Objectives: Hearing loss is most commonly observed at high frequencies. High-frequency hearing loss (HFHL) precedes and predicts hearing loss at lower frequencies. It was previously shown that an automated, self-administered digits-in-noise (DIN) test can be sensitized for detection of HFHL by low-pass filtering the speech-shaped masking noise at 1.5 kHz. This study was designed to investigate whether sensitivity of the DIN to HFHL can be enhanced further using low-pass noise filters with higher cutoff frequencies. Design: US-English digits 0-9, homogenized for audibility, were binaurally presented in different noise maskers including one broadband and three low-pass (cut-off at 2, 4, 8 kHz) filtered speech-shaped noises. DIN-Speech reception thresholds (SRTs) were obtained from 60 normal hearing (NH), and 40 mildly hearing impaired (HI) listeners with bilateral symmetric sensorineural hearing-loss. Standard and extended high frequency audiometric pure tone averages (PTAs) were compared with the DIN-SRTs. Results: Narrower masking noise bandwidth generally produced better (more sensitive) mean DIN-SRTs. There were strong and significant correlations between SRT and PTA in the HI group. Lower frequency, PTALF 0.5,1, 2, 4 kHz had the highest correlation and steepest slope with SRTs obtained from the 2 kHz filter. Higher frequency, PTAHF 4,8,10,12.5 kHz correlated best with SRTs obtained from 4 and 8 kHz filtered noise. The 4 kHz low-pass filter also had the highest sensitivity (92%) and equally-highest (with the 8 kHz filter) specificity (90%) for detecting an average PTAHF of 20 dB or more. Conclusions: Of the filters used, DIN sensitivity to higher frequency hearing loss was greatest using the 4 kHz low-pass filter. These results suggest that low-pass filtered noise may be usefully substituted for broadband noise to improve earlier detection of HFHL using DIN.


2019 ◽  
Vol 11 (11) ◽  
pp. 1125-1133
Author(s):  
Munmun Mondal ◽  
Md. Rafiqul Islam

Fingerprint is becoming the part of our day to day life right from our home to workplace. Now a days for security and safety purpose prime importance is given by it. Also, Fingerprint identification is one of the most popular biometric technologies and which is highly used in criminal investigations, commercial applications, and so on. The performance of a fingerprint image-matching algorithm depends heavily on the quality of the input fingerprint images. It is very important to acquire good quality images. The use of wavelet transform improves the quality of an image and reduces noise level. So, in this research, different compression techniques are used to overcome this problem. Also, we have used different wavelets transformation for compression of fingerprint images. Image quality before compression and after compression are measured by Mean Squared Error (MSE), Signal-to-Noise Ratio (SNR) and Peak Signal-to-Noise Ratio (PSNR). This work is done in MATLAB using DSP and wavelet toolbox. At last, we have compared the filtered noise image method and the compression filtered noise image method.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document