Perception ◽  
1997 ◽  
Vol 26 (1_suppl) ◽  
pp. 70-70
Author(s):  
R Anderson ◽  
M A Georgeson

We investigated orientation coding via the spatial-frequency tuning of the tilt aftereffect (TAE). In the single-adaptation condition, subjects adapted to single gratings of 1 or 8 cycles deg−1, 40% contrast, tilted 15° clockwise or anticlockwise from vertical; in two double-adaptation conditions the 1 and 8 cycles deg−1 gratings were superimposed at opposite orientations (‘plaid’ condition) or at the same orientation (‘parallel’ condition). Test gratings of 1, 2, 4, and 8 cycles deg−1, 20% contrast, were presented for 150 ms in an interleaved staircase procedure that measured the TAE by nulling it, hence making a tilted test grating appear vertical. Initial adaptation was for 3 min, topped up for 2 s between test presentations. Results from the single-grating condition indicated broad spatial-frequency tuning of the TAE, since the effect was still strong when tested three octaves away from the adapter. In the parallel condition, the TAEs were around the average of those reported in the single condition. Negligible TAEs were found in the 1+8 cycles deg−1 plaid condition, indicating that opposing adaptations had effectively cancelled each other out. These findings strengthen the suggestion of Olzak and Thomas (1992 Vision Research32 1885 – 1898) that orientation is encoded via an integrative mechanism which pools or sums the outputs of different spatial-frequency channels, and further imply that much of the adaptation responsible for the TAE occurs at this later broad-band stage.


Perception ◽  
1994 ◽  
Vol 23 (8) ◽  
pp. 957-964 ◽  
Author(s):  
André Delorme

Colour aftereffects were observed in dichoptically viewed achromatic striped patterns after a 25 s period of monocular adaptation to an homogeneous coloured field of red, green, or blue. Three test conditions of dichoptic viewing were used. In condition 1, black line patterns were viewed dichoptically on fused white backgrounds. Stimuli used in condition 2 were similar except that they were white line patterns on black backgrounds. Last, condition 3 was realised with the same stimulus patterns utilised in condition 1, except that the mode of dichoptic viewing produced a juxtaposition rather than a fusion of the two white backgrounds containing the line patterns. Some colour aftereffect was obtained for each colour-adaptation condition and in each test condition. It consisted in a negative colour aftereffect (NCA) in the adapted eye (the colour seen was roughly the complementary of the adaptation colour) and/or a positive colour aftereffect (PCA) in the unadapted eye (the colour seen tended rather to be similar in hue to the adaptation colour). In fact, the following four kinds of responses were obtained: (i) two colour aftereffects, one seen by each eye, ie a NCA involving the adapted eye and a PCA involving the unadapted eye; (ii) a NCA involving the adapted eye only; (iii) a PCA involving the unadapted eye only; (iv) no colour aftereffect at all. Results obtained in different test conditions permitted us to assert that both kinds of colour aftereffect could be produced with white patterns on dark backgrounds as well as with black patterns on white backgrounds and did not require binocular fusion of the white backgrounds. Hypothetical physiological explanations of these aftereffects are available.


1985 ◽  
Vol 57 (2) ◽  
pp. 647-651 ◽  
Author(s):  
William T. Drennen ◽  
Larry L. Rutledge ◽  
William P. Wattles

In the present study the relative effects of biofeedback, a set to relax, and a no set (adaptation) condition upon EMG changes in the frontalis muscles were addressed. Subjects were college student volunteers and the proportion of males to females was equal among groups. All subjects were first monitored for minutes (adaptation, no set). Subsequently, subjects in the biofeedback group were then instructed to relax and also to attend to feedback signals to facilitate relaxation for 20 additional minutes (1-min. trials). Subjects in the relaxation set group were given a set to relax but not biofeedback until the last 5-min. segment. Subjects in the no set group were given no instructions to relax and no biofeedback until the last 5-min. segment. Results indicated reductions of EMG monitored tension levels in all groups with no significant between-group differences. Pre- and poststate anxiety self-report measures also showed a significant reduction in anxiety for all groups but no between-group differences reached significance.


2011 ◽  
Vol 37 (6) ◽  
pp. 697-704 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hakan Bilhan ◽  
Onur Geckili ◽  
Emre Mumcu ◽  
Caglar Bilmenoglu

The aim of this clinical study was to evaluate the prosthodontic maintenance requirements during the first year of service of mandibular overdentures supported by interforaminal implants and to assess the influence of attachment type, implant number, and bite force on these requirements. Fifty-nine patients treated with mandibular implant overdentures between the years 2004 and 2009 and appearing in the 12th-month recall were included in this study. The overdentures constituted 4 groups: 2 single interforaminal implants (1 group with locator and 1 group with ball attachments), 3 single interforaminal implants, 3 splinted interforaminal implants (bar), and 4 splinted interforaminal implants (bar). During the examination, prosthetic parameters such as occlusion, tissue adaptation, condition of the retentive mechanism (matrice and patrice), and the condition of the denture-bearing tissues were evaluated and recorded. No statistically significant relation was found between attachment type, bite force values, implant number, and the occurring complications except the need for relining, which was found significantly more in the ball attachments than in other attachment groups (P  =  .03). After 12 months following the overdenture insertion, there seems to be no relation between occurring complications and patient-related factors, such as maximum bite force, age, and gender, as well as factors related to the overdentures such as number and type of attachments.


1996 ◽  
Vol 76 (1) ◽  
pp. 47-58 ◽  
Author(s):  
D. V. Smith ◽  
H. Liu ◽  
M. B. Vogt

1. The responses of single nucleus of the solitary tract (NST) neurons in the hamster were recorded to an array of Na+ and non-Na+ stimuli under each of three adaptation conditions: distilled H2O, 0.032 M NaCl, and 10 microM amiloride. Each adapting solution flowed for 60 s before delivery of one of seven test stimuli: 0.032 M NaCl, NaNO3, and Na-gluconate, 0.1 M KCl and sucrose, 1 mM HCl, and 3 mM quinine hydrochloride (QHCl). Stimuli were dissolved in distilled H2O (H2O and NaCl adaptation conditions) or 10 microM amiloride (amiloride adaptation condition). 2. Both amiloride treatment and NaCl adaptation reduced responses to the Na+ stimuli. The effects of NaCl adaptation were generally greater than those of amiloride, and the responses to the Na+ salts were reduced by NaCl adaptation in every cell that responded to NaCl, regardless of its best-stimulus classification. Amiloride treatment suppressed the responses to Na+ salts with larger anions (NaNO3 and Na-gluconate) more than the response to NaCl. 3. Unlike amiloride treatment, NaCl adaptation also reduced responses to several non-Na+ stimuli (KCl, HCl, and QHCl). This effect occurred primarily in the NaCl-best neurons that were most highly responsive to NaCl and that showed a postexcitatory suppression after NaCl. This suppression has been observed in recordings from the chorda tympani nerve in both rats and hamsters and in taste receptor cell responses recorded in situ in the rat. If it is a receptor phenomenon, these data would imply that some NaCl-sensitive receptor cells are also responsive to these non-Na+ electrolytes. 4. The effects of amiloride on the responses to Na+ stimuli were not limited to NaCl-best neurons, but occurred in sucrose-best cells as well. These results suggest that the sucrose-best cells in the NST receive converging input from sucrose- and NaCl-best chorda tympani fibers, because there is little Na+ sensitivity in the peripheral sucrose-best fibers and the amiloride sensitivity is restricted to NaCl-best chorda tympani fibers. The responses to NaCl in the few HCl- and QHCl-best NST neurons were not affected by amiloride. 5. Rinsing the tongue with amiloride for 60 s resulted in a reduction in the baseline response rate of NST cells. This effect occurred primarily in NaCl- and sucrose-best NST neurons and implies that much of the spontaneous activity in these brain stem cells arises from amiloride-sensitive channel activity in the peripheral receptor cells. 6. The results of human psychophysical studies show very different effects of NaCl adaptation and amiloride treatment. Adaptation to NaCl produces a robust and specific reduction in the saltiness of all salts. The present results show that NaCl adaptation reduces the responses of all cells sensitive to NaCl. Treatment of the human tongue with amiloride produces a proportionately smaller reduction in the response to NaCl than it does in rodents, and it appears to have no effect on saltiness. Rather, amiloride has been shown to specifically reduce the sour side taste of NaCl, Nagluconate, and LiCl. Therefore conclusions about the effects of amiloride on taste quality based on rodent electrophysiology are questionable.


2009 ◽  
Vol 101 (3) ◽  
pp. 1550-1559 ◽  
Author(s):  
Muriel Panouillères ◽  
Tiffany Weiss ◽  
Christian Urquizar ◽  
Roméo Salemme ◽  
Douglas P. Munoz ◽  
...  

The accuracy of saccadic eye movements is maintained over the long term by adaptation mechanisms that decrease or increase saccade amplitude. It is still unknown whether these opposite adaptive changes rely on common mechanisms. Here, a double-step target paradigm was used to adaptively decrease (backward second target step) or increase (forward step) the amplitude of reactive saccades in one direction only. To test which sensorimotor transformation stages are subjected to these adaptive changes, we measured their transfer to antisaccades in which sensory and motor vectors are spatially dissociated. In the backward adaptation condition, all subjects showed a significant amplitude decrease for adapted prosaccades and a significant transfer of adaptation to antisaccades performed in the adapted direction, but not to oppositely directed antisaccades elicited by a target jump in the adapted direction. In the forward adaptation condition, only 14 of 19 subjects showed a significant amplitude increase for prosaccades and no significant adaptation transfer to antisaccades was detected in either the adapted or nonadapted direction. These findings suggest that, whereas the level(s) of forward adaptation cannot be resolved, the mechanisms involved in backward adaptation of reactive saccades take place at a sensorimotor level downstream from the vector inversion process of antisaccades and differ markedly from those involved in forward adaptation.


PLoS ONE ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 16 (12) ◽  
pp. e0261927
Author(s):  
Xiaohua Zhuang ◽  
Tam Tran ◽  
Doris Jin ◽  
Riya Philip ◽  
Chaorong Wu

Contrast sensitivity is reduced in older adults and is often measured at an overall perceptual level. Recent human psychophysical studies have provided paradigms to measure contrast sensitivity independently in the magnocellular (MC) and parvocellular (PC) visual pathways and have reported desensitization in the MC pathway after flicker adaptation. The current study investigates the influence of aging on contrast sensitivity and on the desensitization effect in the two visual pathways. The steady- and pulsed-pedestal paradigms were used to measure contrast sensitivity under two adaptation conditions in 45 observers. In the non-flicker adaptation condition, observers adapted to a pedestal array of four 1°×1° squares presented with a steady luminance; in the flicker adaptation condition, observers adapted to a square-wave modulated luminance flicker of 7.5 Hz and 50% contrast. Results showed significant age-related contrast sensitivity reductions in the MC and PC pathways, with a significantly larger decrease of contrast sensitivity for individuals older than 50 years of age in the MC pathway but not in the PC pathway. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that sensitivity reduction observed at the overall perceptual level likely comes from both the MC and PC visual pathways, with a more dramatic reduction resulting from the MC pathway for adults >50 years of age. In addition, a similar desensitization effect from flicker adaptation was observed in the MC pathway for all ages, which suggests that aging may not affect the process of visual adaptation to rapid luminance flicker.


Author(s):  
S. A. Bugorkova ◽  
T. V. Bugorkova ◽  
V. V. Kutyrev

Worked out is the algorithm of cholera vaccines quality based on application of the methods of quantitative registration of formalized parameters which characterize pathologic and adaptive processes in the intestine of the immunized laboratory animals. The following parameters were selected as formalized ones: quantity of interepithelial lymphocytes, morpho-functional condition of apudocytes and scyphiform cells. Morphometric parameters are presented as associations of indicators in the form of indices and coefficients in order to carry out interrelated assessment of separate parts of homeostasis. It was determined that in case the assessed parameters were in the limits identified in corresponding controls, adaptation condition of the organism and alterations described in histological assay were considered as benign and characterized safety or efficiency of tested cholera vaccines.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document