Memory Reorganization by Synaptic Protein Degradation

2009 ◽  
Vol 24 (S1) ◽  
pp. 1-1
Author(s):  
B.-K. Kaang

An accumulating body of evidence shows that the retrieval process of long-term memory is not static and requires de novo protein synthesis. Thus long-term memories are dynamic and particularly become fragile during its retrieval. Importantly, memory retrieval is regarded as a step necessary for incorporating new information into preexisting memories. We have examined whether protein degradation is involved in the memory reorganization or not. In this presentation I will present the evidence that synaptic proteins are degraded by polyubiquitination and proteasome pathway in the hippocampus after the retrieval of contextual fear conditioning. In addition, we found that the infusion of a proteasome inhibitor into the hippocampus prevented the memory impairment induced by anisomycin, a protein synthesis inhibitor. This indicates that ubiquitin/proteasome-dependent protein degradation is involved in destabilization processes accompanying the memory retrieval. It also supports our hypothesis that preexisting memory is disrupted by synaptic protein degradation before updated memory is strengthened by protein synthesis. Our data also showed that synaptic protein degradation plays a critical role in fear memory extinction, a simple form of memory reorganization. Taken together, synaptic protein degradation is critically involved in the reorganization of the preexisting memories.

2015 ◽  
Vol 26 (5) ◽  
pp. 507-546 ◽  
Author(s):  
Abdul-Karim Abbas ◽  
Agnès Villers ◽  
Laurence Ris

AbstractLong-term potentiation (LTP) remains the most widely accepted model for learning and memory. In accordance with this belief, the temporal differentiation of LTP into early and late phases is accepted as reflecting the differentiation of short-term and long-term memory. Moreover, during the past 30 years, protein synthesis inhibitors have been used to separate the early, protein synthesis-independent (E-LTP) phase and the late, protein synthesis-dependent (L-LTP) phase. However, the role of these proteins has not been formally identified. Additionally, several reports failed to show an effect of protein synthesis inhibitors on LTP. In this review, a detailed analysis of extensive behavioral and electrophysiological data reveals that the presumed correspondence of LTP temporal phases to memory phases is neither experimentally nor theoretically consistent. Moreover, an overview of the time courses of E-LTP in hippocampal slices reveals a wide variability ranging from <1 h to more than 5 h. The existence of all these conflictual findings should lead to a new vision of LTP. We believe that the E-LTP vs. L-LTP distinction, established with protein synthesis inhibitor studies, reflects a false dichotomy. We suggest that the duration of LTP and its dependency on protein synthesis are related to the availability of a set of proteins at synapses and not to the de novo synthesis of plasticity-related proteins. This availability is determined by protein turnover kinetics, which is regulated by previous and ongoing electrical activities and by energy store availability.


1998 ◽  
Vol 5 (4) ◽  
pp. 365-374 ◽  
Author(s):  
Roussoudan Bourtchouladze ◽  
Ted Abel ◽  
Nathaniel Berman ◽  
Rachael Gordon ◽  
Kyle Lapidus ◽  
...  

We have used a combined genetic and pharmacological approach to define the time course of the requirement for protein kinase A (PKA) and protein synthesis in long-term memory for contextual fear conditioning in mice. The time course of amnesia in transgenic mice that express R(AB) and have genetically reduced PKA activity in the hippocampus parallels that observed both in mice treated with inhibitors of PKA and mice treated with inhibitors of protein synthesis. This PKA- and protein synthesis-dependent memory develops between 1 hr and 3 hr after training. By injecting the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin or the PKA inhibitor Rp-cAMPs at various times after training, we find that depending on the nature of training, contextual memory has either one or two brief consolidation periods requiring synthesis of new proteins, and each of these also requires PKA. Weak training shows two time periods of sensitivity to inhibitors of protein synthesis and PKA, whereas stronger training exhibits only one. These studies underscore the parallel dependence of long-term contextual memory on protein synthesis and PKA and suggest that different training protocols may recruit a common signaling pathway in distinct ways.


2002 ◽  
Vol 357 (1420) ◽  
pp. 521-529 ◽  
Author(s):  
Shao Jun Tang ◽  
Erin M. Schuman

In neurons, many proteins that are involved in the transduction of synaptic activity and the expression of neural plasticity are specifically localized at synapses. How these proteins are targeted is not clearly understood. One mechanism is synaptic protein synthesis. According to this idea, messenger RNA (mRNA) translation from the polyribosomes that are observed at the synaptic regions provides a local source of synaptic proteins. Although an increasing number of mRNA species has been detected in the dendrite, information about the synaptic synthesis of specific proteins in a physiological context is still limited. The physiological function of synaptic synthesis of specific proteins in synaptogenesis and neural plasticity expression remains to be shown. Experiments aimed at understanding the mechanisms and functions f synaptic protein synthesis might provide important information about the molecular nature of neural plasticity.


2017 ◽  
Author(s):  
Donal T. Skelly ◽  
Éadaoin W. Griffin ◽  
Carol L. Murray ◽  
Sarah Harney ◽  
Conor O’Boyle ◽  
...  

AbstractSystemic inflammation can impair cognition with relevance to dementia, delirium and post-operative cognitive dysfunction. Acute episodes of delirium also contribute significantly to rates of long-term cognitive decline, implying that de novo pathology occurs during these acute episodes. Whether systemic inflammation-induced acute dysfunction and acute brain injury occur by overlapping or discrete mechanisms has not been investigated. Here we show that systemic inflammation, induced by bacterial LPS, produces both working memory deficits and acute brain injury in the degenerating brain and that these occur by dissociable IL-1-dependent processes. In normal C57BL/6 mice, LPS (100μg/kg) did not affect working memory but robustly impaired contextual fear conditioning (CFC). However prior hippocampal synaptic loss left mice selectively vulnerable to LPS-induced working memory deficits. Systemically administered IL-1 receptor antagonist (IL-1RA) was protective against, and systemic IL-1β replicated, these working memory deficits. Although LPS-induced deficits still occured in IL-1RI-/- mice, systemic TNF-α was sufficient to induce similar deficits, indicating redundancy among these cytokines. Dexamethasone abolished systemic cytokine synthesis and was protective against working memory deficits despite failing to block brain IL-1β synthesis. Direct application of IL-1β to ex vivo hippocampal slices induced non-synaptic depolarisation and irrevesible loss of membrane potential in CA1 neurons from diseased animals and systemic LPS increased apoptosis in the degenerating brain, in an IL-1RI-/- dependent-fashion. The data suggest that LPS induces working memory dysfunction via circulating IL-1β but dysfunction leading to neuronal death is mediated by hippocampal IL-1β. The data suggest that acute systemic inflammation produces both reversible cognitive deficits, resembling delirium, and acute brain injury that may lead to long-term cognitive impairment but that these events are mechanistically dissociable. This would have significant implications for management of cognitive dysfunction and decline during acute illness.


1998 ◽  
Vol 274 (1) ◽  
pp. R120-R125 ◽  
Author(s):  
Tohru Sakakibara ◽  
Thomas H. Hintze ◽  
Alberto Nasjletti

We studied the determinants of kinin release into the venous effluent of rat hindquarters perfused with Krebs bicarbonate buffer. Kinin release in preparations perfused with control media (14.6 ± 2.5–20.7 ± 6.7 pg/15 min) was surpassed by that in preparations perfused with media containing kininase inhibitors (243 ± 53 to 276 ± 78 pg/15 min). Kinin release increased when purified kininogen (from 242 ± 43 to 3,365 ± 725 pg/15 min) or kallikrein (from 270 ± 49 to 30,649 ± 8,040 pg/15 min) was added to the perfusate. Conversely, kinin release fell when the kallikrein inhibitor aprotinin (from 272 ± 58 to 122 ± 27 pg/15 min) or soybean trypsin inhibitor (from 273 ± 52 to 195 ± 25 pg/15 min) was added. Both basal and kininogen-induced kinin release were attenuated in preparations perfused with media containing cycloheximide, a protein synthesis inhibitor, but kallikrein-induced kinin release was not. These data suggest that kinin release from perfused rat hindquarters reflects the activity of both the kinin-degrading and kinin-generating pathways and that the latter is sustained by a kallikrein manufactured de novo and by preexistent kininogen(s).


2020 ◽  
Vol 21 (15) ◽  
pp. 5537
Author(s):  
Johannes Kornhuber ◽  
Iulia Zoicas

It is well known that long-term consolidation of newly acquired information, including information related to social fear, require de novo protein synthesis. However, the temporal dynamics of protein synthesis during the consolidation of social fear memories is unclear. To address this question, mice received a single systemic injection with the protein synthesis inhibitor, anisomycin, at different time-points before or after social fear conditioning (SFC), and memory was assessed 24 h later. We showed that anisomycin impaired the consolidation of social fear memories in a time-point-dependent manner. Mice that received anisomycin 20 min before, immediately after, 6 h, or 8 h after SFC showed reduced expression of social fear, indicating impaired social fear memory, whereas anisomycin caused no effects when administered 4 h after SFC. These results suggest that consolidation of social fear memories requires two stages of protein synthesis: (1) an initial stage starting during or immediately after SFC, and (2) a second stage starting around 6 h after SFC and lasting for at least 5 h.


1990 ◽  
Vol 110 (4) ◽  
pp. 1427-1438 ◽  
Author(s):  
S Aznavoorian ◽  
M L Stracke ◽  
H Krutzsch ◽  
E Schiffmann ◽  
L A Liotta

Transduction of signals initiating motility by extracellular matrix (ECM) molecules differed depending on the type of matrix molecule and whether the ligand was in solution or bound to a substratum. Laminin, fibronectin, and type IV collagen stimulated both chemotaxis and haptotaxis of the A2058 human melanoma cell line. Peak chemotactic responses were reached at 50-200 nM for laminin, 50-100 nM for fibronectin, and 200-370 nM for type IV collagen. Checkerboard analysis of each attractant in solution demonstrated a predominantly directional (chemotactic) response, with a minor chemokinetic component. The cells also migrated in a concentration-dependent manner to insoluble step gradients of substratum-bound attractant (haptotaxis). The haptotactic responses reached maximal levels at coating concentrations of 20 nM for laminin and type IV collagen, and from 30 to 45 nM for fibronectin. Pretreatment of cells with the protein synthesis inhibitor, cycloheximide (5 micrograms/ml), resulted in a 5-30% inhibition of both chemotactic and haptotactic responses to each matrix protein, indicating that de novo protein synthesis was not required for a significant motility response. Pretreatment of cells with 50-500 micrograms/ml of synthetic peptides containing the fibronectin cell-recognition sequence GRGDS resulted in a concentration-dependent inhibition of fibronectin-mediated chemotaxis and haptotaxis (70-80% inhibition compared to control motility); negative control peptide GRGES had only a minimal effect. Neither GRGDS nor GRGES significantly inhibited motility to laminin or type IV collagen. Therefore, these results support a role for the RGD-directed integrin receptor in both types of motility response to fibronectin. After pretreatment with pertussis toxin (PT), chemotactic responses to laminin, fibronectin, and type IV collagen were distinctly different. Chemotaxis to laminin was intermediate in sensitivity; chemotaxis to fibronectin was completely insensitive; and chemotaxis to type IV collagen was profoundly inhibited by PT. In marked contrast to the inhibition of chemotaxis, the hepatotactic responses to all three ligands were unaffected by any of the tested concentrations of PT. High concentrations of cholera toxin (CT; 10 micrograms/ml) or the cAMP analogue, 8-Br-cAMP (0.5 mM), did not significantly affect chemotactic or haptotactic motility to any of the attractant proteins, ruling out the involvement of cAMP in the biochemical pathway initiating motility in these cells. The sensitivity of chemotaxis induced by laminin and type IV collagen, but not fibronectin, to PT indicates the involvement of a PT-sensitive G protein in transduction of the signals initiating motility to soluble laminin and type IV collagen.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)


1971 ◽  
Vol 178 (1053) ◽  
pp. 455-464 ◽  

1. Memory in day-old-chickens during the first few hours after learning can be made to decline by the prior intracranial injection of two classes of drugs. 2. Sodium pump inhibitors in increasing doses cause increasingly rapid loss of memory. 3. Protein synthesis inhibitors in increasing doses attain a maximum potency in causing memory decline and the rate may not be further accelerated by higher doses. 4. Adding a sodium pump inhibitor to the inhibition of protein synthesis increases memory loss. 5. Adding a protein synthesis inhibitor to a sodium pump inhibitor causes no further loss. 6. Therefore within a few minutes of learning a short-term memory of limited time span but independent of protein synthesis becomes supplemented and eventually replaced by a long-term storage requiring protein synthesis. The amount of long-term store is set by the amount of short-term memory. 7. The short-term store could be directly dependent on post-activation enhancement of Na + extrusion from neurons. Some physiological mechanisms by which this could be achieved and how this might activate protein synthesis are discussed.


F1000Research ◽  
2017 ◽  
Vol 6 ◽  
pp. 675 ◽  
Author(s):  
Laurie D. Cohen ◽  
Noam E. Ziv

Maintaining synaptic integrity and function depends on the continuous removal and degradation of aged or damaged proteins. Synaptic protein degradation has received considerable attention in the context of synaptic plasticity and growing interest in relation to neurodegenerative and other disorders. Conversely, less attention has been given to constitutive, ongoing synaptic protein degradation and the roles canonical degradation pathways play in these processes. Here we briefly review recent progress on this topic and new experimental approaches which have expedited such progress and highlight several emerging principles. These include the realization that synaptic proteins typically have unusually long lifetimes, as might be expected from the remote locations of most synaptic sites; the possibility that degradation pathways can change with time from synthesis, cellular context, and physiological input; and that degradation pathways, other than ubiquitin-proteasomal-mediated degradation, might play key roles in constitutive protein degradation at synaptic sites. Finally, we point to the importance of careful experimental design and sufficiently sensitive techniques for studying synaptic protein degradation, which bring into account their slow turnover rates and complex life cycles.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document