Preschool Fantasy-Reality Discrimination: Influences of Trait and Primed Fearfulness

2017 ◽  
Vol 178 (2) ◽  
pp. 133-138
Author(s):  
Aleksandra V. Petkova ◽  
Kathleen M. Cain
2014 ◽  
Vol 45 (3) ◽  
pp. 389-395 ◽  
Author(s):  
David Smailes ◽  
Elizabeth Meins ◽  
Charles Fernyhough

2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
David Smailes ◽  
Emma Burdis ◽  
Constantina Gregoriou ◽  
Bryony Fenton ◽  
Rob Dudley

Introduction: It has been proposed that hallucinations occur because of problems with reality discrimination (when internal, self-generated cognitions are misattributed to an external, non-self source) and because of elevated levels of top-down processing. In this study, we examined whether visual reality discrimination abilities and elevated top-down processing (assessed via face pareidolia-proneness) were associated with how often non-clinical participants report visual hallucination-like experiences. Methods: Participants (N = 82, mean age = 23.12 years) completed a visual reality discrimination task and a face pareidolia task, as well as self-report measures of schizotypy and of the frequency of visual hallucination-like experiences. Results: Regression analysis demonstrated that the number of false alarms made on the visual reality discrimination task and the number of hits made on the face pareidolia task were independent predictors of the frequency of visual hallucination-like experiences. Correlations between performance on the task and levels of schizotypy were not statistically significant.Conclusions: These findings suggest that weaker visual reality discrimination abilities and elevated levels of top-down processing are associated with visual hallucination-proneness and are discussed in terms of the idea that clinical visual hallucinations and non-clinical visual hallucination-like experiences share similar cognitive mechanisms. This is a preprint of an article published by Taylor & Francis in Cognitive Neuropsychiatry on 06/DEC/2019, available at the following doi: 10.1080/13546805.2019.1700789


2000 ◽  
Vol 39 (4) ◽  
pp. 397-406 ◽  
Author(s):  
Koen B. E. Böker ◽  
Ron Hijman ◽  
René S. Kahn ◽  
Edward H. F. Haan

2008 ◽  
Vol 47 (3) ◽  
pp. 323-334 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sandra Bucci ◽  
Mike Startup ◽  
Paula Wynn ◽  
Andrew Heathcote ◽  
Amanda Baker ◽  
...  

1995 ◽  
Vol 15 (2) ◽  
pp. 113-131 ◽  
Author(s):  
Robert G. Kunzendorf ◽  
Cindy Moran ◽  
Reid Gray

One hundred seventy-seven college students completed personality measures including the MMPI-168, and reality-discrimination measures including a timed discrimination task developed by the first author. Previously on such a timed task, normal subjects discriminated percepts more quickly from images of perceptual vividness than from faint images, as if they registered more “central innervation” during more vivid imaging. Presently, on each of the four practice trials and forty-four timed trials in Task 1, subjects fixated on a dot, perceived a stimulus to one side, imaged an identical stimulus on the other side and rated its vividness while continuing to image. Then, either the dot became a P and subjects pressed a button on the side of the percept, as quickly as possible, or the dot became an I and subjects pressed a button on the side of the image. The slope, within subject, of image/percept discrimination times over image-vividness ratings was computed for the 149 subjects who rated some of their forty-four images more vividly than others. It was predicted and found that self-described hallucinators and MMPI-defined paranoids discriminated percepts less quickly from vivid images, as if the greater “central innervation” behind more vivid images is not registered by psychosis-prone subjects.


2019 ◽  
Vol 25 (2) ◽  
pp. 113-125 ◽  
Author(s):  
David Smailes ◽  
Emma Burdis ◽  
Constantina Gregoriou ◽  
Bryony Fenton ◽  
Rob Dudley

2011 ◽  
Vol 42 (5) ◽  
pp. 1025-1036 ◽  
Author(s):  
F. Varese ◽  
E. Barkus ◽  
R. P. Bentall

BackgroundIt has been proposed that the relationship between childhood trauma and hallucinations can be explained by dissociative processes. The present study examined whether the effect of childhood trauma on hallucination-proneness is mediated by dissociative tendencies. In addition, the influence of dissociative symptoms on a cognitive process believed to underlie hallucinatory experiences (i.e. reality discrimination; the capacity to discriminate between internal and external cognitive events) was also investigated.MethodPatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders (n=45) and healthy controls (with no history of hallucinations;n=20) completed questionnaire measures of hallucination-proneness, dissociative tendencies and childhood trauma, as well as performing an auditory signal detection task.ResultsCompared to both healthy and non-hallucinating clinical controls, hallucinating patients reported both significantly higher dissociative tendencies and childhood sexual abuse. Dissociation positively mediated the effect of childhood trauma on hallucination-proneness. This mediational role was particularly robust for sexual abuse over other types of trauma. Signal detection abnormalities were evident in hallucinating patients and patients with a history of hallucinations, but were not associated with pathological dissociative symptoms.ConclusionsThese results are consistent with dissociative accounts of the trauma-hallucinations link. Dissociation, however, does not affect reality discrimination. Future research should examine whether other cognitive processes associated with both dissociative states and hallucinations (e.g. deficits in cognitive inhibition) may explain the relationship between dissociation and hallucinatory experiences.


2020 ◽  
Vol 11 (3sup1) ◽  
pp. 146-152
Author(s):  
Mirona Letiția Dobri ◽  
◽  
Alina-Ioana Voinea ◽  
Codrina Moraru ◽  
Petronela Nechita ◽  
...  

From the beginning of time, the layman always described and understood psychosis as a dream-like state. Researchers have characterized both psychosis and dreaming with common denominators, both displaying visuomotor hallucinations, loose associations, metacognitive deficit, impaired reality discrimination, strong emotional component, resulting in a general lack of insight. The association of psychosis with dreaming was present in literature centuries ago, in the works of great thinkers such as Kant and Schopenhauer, which prompted many modern specialists to delve deeper into studying this connection for a better understanding of psychosis and possible applications in the clinical practice. During REM sleep, which is the most associated with dreaming, visual and motor areas in the brain show increased activation, which is congruent with the presence of hallucinations, the hallmark of the dreaming state. Also, the amygdala, involved in emotion regulation, has a prominent role in the sleeping brain. The deactivation of parts of the prefrontal cortex translate in altered capacity for making decisions and critical thinking. Several neuroimaging studies have shown similar neural patterns in the wakeful state of psychotic patients, especially those associated with the presence or absence of insight. As insight is thought to play a major role in treatment compliance and quality of life in psychotic patients, it is the most studied element linking psychosis and REM sleep. Lucid dreaming is a state of awareness of dreaming, while the individual is still asleep. The dreamer has a degree of control of the narrative and capacity for self-reflection, aspects deemed as insight. Lucid dreaming is a rare occurrence, but has the potential to be trained, concept with great relevance in researching modalities for insight gain in psychotic patients. In conclusion, the research of insight present in lucid dreaming shows great prospect for developing better interventions that target the lack of it in psychotic patients, thus contributing to significant improvement in their prognosis, quality of life and treatment compliance.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document