Transmission through the Dorsal Spinocerebellar and Spinoreticular Tracts

2002 ◽  
Vol 97 (5) ◽  
pp. 1178-1188 ◽  
Author(s):  
Peter J. Soja ◽  
Niwat Taepavarapruk ◽  
Walton Pang ◽  
Brian E. Cairns ◽  
Shelly A. McErlane ◽  
...  

Background Most of what is known regarding the actions of injectable barbiturate anesthetics on the activity of lumbar sensory neurons arises from experiments performed in acute animal preparations that are exposed to invasive surgery and neural depression caused by coadministered inhalational anesthetics. Other parameters such as cortical synchronization and motor ouflow are typically not monitored, and, therefore, anesthetic actions on multiple cellular systems have not been quantitatively compared. Methods The activities of antidromically identified dorsal spinocerebellar and spinoreticular tract neurons, neck motoneurons, and cortical neurons were monitored extracellularly before, during, and following recovery from the anesthetic state induced by thiopental in intact, chronically instrumented animal preparations. Results Intravenous administration of 15 mg/kg, but not 5 mg/kg, of thiopental to awake cats induced general anesthesia that was characterized by 5-10 min of cortical synchronization, reflected as large-amplitude slow-wave events and neck muscle atonia. However, even though the animal behaviorally began to reemerge from the anesthetic state after this 5-10-min period, neck muscle (neck motoneuron) activity recovered more slowly and remained significantly suppressed for up to 23 min after thiopental administration. The spontaneous activity of both dorsal spinocerebellar and spinoreticular tract neurons was maximally suppressed 5 min after administration but remained significantly attenuated for up to 17 min after injection. Peripheral nerve and glutamate-evoked responses of dorsal spinocerebellar and spinoreticular tract neurons were particularly sensitive to thiopental administration and remained suppressed for up to 20 min after injection. Conclusions These results demonstrate that thiopental administration is associated with a prolonged blockade of motoneuron output and sensory transmission through the dorsal spinocerebellar and spinoreticular tracts that exceeds the duration of general anesthesia. Further, the blockade of glutamate-evoked neuronal responses indicates that these effects are due, in part, to a local action of the drug in the spinal cord. The authors suggest that this combination of lumbar sensory and motoneuron inhibition underlies the prolonged impairment of reflex coordination observed when thiopental is used clinically.

2005 ◽  
Vol 94 (2) ◽  
pp. 979-989 ◽  
Author(s):  
Brian J. Mickey ◽  
John C. Middlebrooks

We recorded unit activity in the auditory cortex (fields A1, A2, and PAF) of anesthetized cats while presenting paired clicks with variable locations and interstimulus delays (ISDs). In human listeners, such sounds elicit the precedence effect, in which localization of the lagging sound is impaired at ISDs ≲10 ms. In the present study, neurons typically responded to the leading stimulus with a brief burst of spikes, followed by suppression lasting 100–200 ms. At an ISD of 20 ms, at which listeners report a distinct lagging sound, only 12% of units showed discrete lagging responses. Long-lasting suppression was found in all sampled cortical fields, for all leading and lagging locations, and at all sound levels. Recordings from awake cats confirmed this long-lasting suppression in the absence of anesthesia, although recovery from suppression was faster in the awake state. Despite the lack of discrete lagging responses at delays of 1–20 ms, the spike patterns of 40% of units varied systematically with ISD, suggesting that many neurons represent lagging sounds implicitly in their temporal firing patterns rather than explicitly in discrete responses. We estimated the amount of location-related information transmitted by spike patterns at delays of 1–16 ms under conditions in which we varied only the leading location or only the lagging location. Consistent with human psychophysical results, transmission of information about the leading location was high at all ISDs. Unlike listeners, however, transmission of information about the lagging location remained low, even at ISDs of 12–16 ms.


2008 ◽  
Vol 100 (3) ◽  
pp. 1622-1634 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ling Qin ◽  
JingYu Wang ◽  
Yu Sato

Previous studies in anesthetized animals reported that the primary auditory cortex (A1) showed homogenous phasic responses to FM tones, namely a transient response to a particular instantaneous frequency when FM sweeps traversed a neuron's tone-evoked receptive field (TRF). Here, in awake cats, we report that A1 cells exhibit heterogeneous FM responses, consisting of three patterns. The first is continuous firing when a slow FM sweep traverses the receptive field of a cell with a sustained tonal response. The duration and amplitude of FM response decrease with increasing sweep speed. The second pattern is transient firing corresponding to the cell's phasic tonal response. This response could be evoked only by a fast FM sweep through the cell's TRF, suggesting a preference for fast FM. The third pattern was associated with the off response to pure tones and was composed of several discrete response peaks during slow FM stimulus. These peaks were not predictable from the cell's tonal response but reliably reflected the time when FM swept across specific frequencies. Our A1 samples often exhibited a complex response pattern, combining two or three of the basic patterns above, resulting in a heterogeneous response population. The diversity of FM responses suggests that A1 use multiple mechanisms to fully represent the whole range of FM parameters, including frequency extent, sweep speed, and direction.


eLife ◽  
2016 ◽  
Vol 5 ◽  
Author(s):  
Bram-Ernst Verhoef ◽  
John HR Maunsell

Shifting attention among visual stimuli at different locations modulates neuronal responses in heterogeneous ways, depending on where those stimuli lie within the receptive fields of neurons. Yet how attention interacts with the receptive-field structure of cortical neurons remains unclear. We measured neuronal responses in area V4 while monkeys shifted their attention among stimuli placed in different locations within and around neuronal receptive fields. We found that attention interacts uniformly with the spatially-varying excitation and suppression associated with the receptive field. This interaction explained the large variability in attention modulation across neurons, and a non-additive relationship among stimulus selectivity, stimulus-induced suppression and attention modulation that has not been previously described. A spatially-tuned normalization model precisely accounted for all observed attention modulations and for the spatial summation properties of neurons. These results provide a unified account of spatial summation and attention-related modulation across both the classical receptive field and the surround.


1985 ◽  
Vol 54 (3) ◽  
pp. 578-591 ◽  
Author(s):  
G. E. Loeb ◽  
J. A. Hoffer ◽  
W. B. Marks

Chronically implanted electrodes were used to record the activity of identified single muscle spindle afferents in awake cats during responses to various types of manual and electrical stimulation. During vigorous cyclical responses such as shaking and scratching, spindle afferents generally maintained at least some activity during both lengthening and shortening of the parent muscle, indicating that the programs for these movements include both extra- and intrafusal recruitment. During noncyclical responses such as ipsilateral limb withdrawal and crossed-extension, spindle activity was modest and poorly correlated with extrafusal activity. Weak cutaneous nerve shocks during walking elicited complex excitatory and inhibitory phase-dependent reflexes in the various muscles studied but caused relatively little change in spindle afferent activity, indicating a lack of correlation between alpha and gamma motoneuron activity. A primary and a secondary afferent from sartorius muscle were recorded simultaneously during walking cycles that were perturbed by electrically induced twitches of the antagonist hamstring muscles; both demonstrated highly sensitive, short latency responses to the resulting skeletal motion, consistent with their previously suggested roles in detecting small brief mechanical perturbations. The degree to which fusimotor responses were correlated with extrafusal responses to somatosensory perturbations was highly dependent on the specific nature of the stimulus and the response. Fusimotor reprogramming of the spindle sensitivity appears to be a feature of cyclical movements that are presumably under proprioceptive control, whereas brief perturbations within the context of a particular motor program may be ignored by the fusimotor system.


2021 ◽  
Vol 12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Guofang Shen ◽  
Wei-Xing Shi

Cortical neurons oscillate between Up and Down states during slow wave sleep and general anesthesia. Recent studies show that Up/Down oscillations also occur during quiet wakefulness. Arousal eliminates Down states and transforms Up/Down oscillations to a persistent Up state. Further evidence suggests that Up/Down oscillations are crucial to memory consolidation, whereas their transition to a persistent Up state is essential for arousal and attention. We have shown that D-amphetamine promotes cortical Up state, and the effect depends on activation of central α1A adrenergic receptors. Here, we report that dopamine also plays a role in D-amphetamine’s effect. Thus, using local-field-potential recording in the prefrontal cortex in chloral hydrate-anesthetized rats, we showed that the Up-state promoting effect of D-amphetamine was attenuated by antagonists at either D1 or D2-like dopamine receptors. The effect was also partially mimicked by co-activation of D1 and D2-like receptors. These results are consistent with the fact that D-amphetamine increases the release of both norepinephrine and dopamine. They are also in agreement with studies showing that dopamine promotes wakefulness and mediates D-amphetamine-induced emergence from general anesthesia. The effect of D-amphetamine was not mimicked, however, by activation of either D1 or D2-like receptors alone, indicating an interdependence between D1 and D2-like receptors. The dopamine/norepinephrine precursor L-DOPA also failed to promote the Up state. While more studies are needed to understand the difference between L-DOPA and D-amphetamine, our finding may provide an explanation for why L-DOPA lacks significant psychostimulant properties and is ineffective in treating attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
David St-Amand ◽  
Curtis L Baker

Neurons in the primary visual cortex (V1) receive excitation and inhibition from two different pathways processing lightness (ON) and darkness (OFF). V1 neurons overall respond more strongly to dark than light stimuli (Yeh, Xing and Shapley, 2010; Kremkow et al., 2014), consistent with a preponderance of darker regions in natural images (Ratliff et al., 2010), as well as human psychophysics (Buchner & Baumgartner, 2007). However, it has been unclear whether this "dark-dominance" is due to more excitation from the OFF pathway (Jin et al., 2008) or more inhibition from the ON pathway (Taylor et al., 2018). To understand the mechanisms behind dark-dominance, we record electrophysiological responses of individual simple-type V1 neurons to natural image stimuli and then train biologically inspired convolutional neural networks to predict the neuronal responses. Analyzing a sample of 74 neurons (in anesthetized, paralyzed cats) has revealed their responses to be more driven by dark than light stimuli, consistent with previous investigations (Yeh et al., 2010; Kremkow et al., 2013). We show this asymmetry to be predominantly due to slower inhibition to dark stimuli rather than by stronger excitation from the thalamocortical OFF pathway. Consistent with dark-dominant neurons having faster responses than light-dominant neurons (Komban et al., 2014), we find dark-dominance to solely occur in the early latencies of neuronal responses. Neurons that are strongly dark-dominated also tend to be less orientation selective. This novel approach gives us new insight into the dark-dominance phenomenon and provides an avenue to address new questions about excitatory and inhibitory integration in cortical neurons.


2006 ◽  
Vol 96 (5) ◽  
pp. 2377-2387 ◽  
Author(s):  
C. Elizabeth Boudreau ◽  
Tori H. Williford ◽  
John H. R. Maunsell

Spatial attention improves performance at attended locations and correspondingly modulates firing rates of cortical neurons. The size of these behavioral and neuronal effects depends on the difficulty of the task performed at the attended location. Psychological theorists have attributed this to a tighter focus of a fixed amount of processing resource at the attended location, but the effects of task difficulty on the distribution of neuronal effects of attention across the visual field have not been fully explored. We trained rhesus monkeys to do a detection task in which difficulty and spatial attention were manipulated independently. Probe stimuli were used to measure behavioral performance in different conditions of attention and difficulty. Animals performed better at attended locations and this advantage increased with difficulty, consistent with data from human psychophysics. Neuronal modulation by spatial attention was larger with greater difficulty. In two animals, increasing difficulty caused a modest increase in neuronal responses to visual stimuli regardless of the locus of spatial attention. In a third animal, which was previously trained to ignore multiple distracting stimuli, increasing task difficulty increased responses at the focus of attention and suppressed responses away from the focus of attention. The results show that difficulty can modulate effects of spatial attention in V4; it can alter the distribution of sensory responses across the visual scene in ways that may depend on the subject's behavioral strategy.


2009 ◽  
Vol 102 (2) ◽  
pp. 700-713 ◽  
Author(s):  
Dhananjay R. Namjoshi ◽  
Shelly A. McErlane ◽  
Niwat Taepavarapruk ◽  
Peter J. Soja

The recent discovery of a barbiturate-sensitive “general anesthesia switch” mechanism localized in the rat brain stem mesopontine tegmental anesthesia area (MPTA) has challenged the current view of the nonspecific actions of general anesthetic agents in the CNS. In this study we provide electrophysiological evidence that the antinociception, which accompanies the behavioral state resembling general anesthesia following pentobarbital (PB) microinjections into the MPTA of awake rats, could be accompanied by the attenuation of sensory transmission through the spinothalamic tract (STT). Following bilateral microinjections of PB into the MPTA spontaneous firing rate (SFR), antidromic firing index (FI), and sciatic (Sc) as well as sural (Su) nerve-evoked responses (ER) of identified lumbar STT neurons in the isoflurane-anesthetized rat were quantified using extracellular recording techniques. Microinjections of PB into the MPTA significantly suppressed the SFR (47%), magnitudes of Sc- (26%) and Su-ER (36%), and FI (41%) of STT neurons. Microinjections of PB-free vehicle control did not alter any of the above-cited electrophysiological parameters. The results from this study suggest that antinociception, which occurs during the anesthesia-like state following PB microinjections into the MPTA, may be due, in part, to (in)direct inhibition of STT neurons via switching mechanism(s) located in the MPTA. This study provides a provenance for investigating electrophysiologically the actions on STT neurons of other current agents used clinically to maintain the state of general anesthesia.


1996 ◽  
Vol 75 (1) ◽  
pp. 496-507 ◽  
Author(s):  
J. W. McClurkin ◽  
J. A. Zarbock ◽  
L. M. Optican

1. In the previous paper we reported our analysis of the responses of neurons in cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 to a set of stimuli that consisted of all 36 combinations of six colors and six patterns. Neurons in all three cortical areas simultaneously encoded information about both the color and pattern of the stimulus in the number and temporal distribution of spikes in their responses. To account for this ability, we propose that a neuron's response consists of separable temporal codes representing the color and pattern of the stimulus that are multiplexed together. 2. We used nonlinear regression to fit the model parameters to the data. We used the responses to 30 of the 36 stimuli as a training set to estimate the parameters of the model and the responses to the remaining 6 stimuli as a test set. After training, the model fitted the responses to stimuli in the training sets very well and predicted the responses to stimuli in the test sets. Thus neuronal responses to colored patterns contain separate temporal codes representing color and pattern. 3. After establishing the model parameters, we obtained the waveforms that represented each neuron's temporal codes for the six colors and six patterns of our stimulus set. We then proceeded with a series of analyses to determine whether these waveforms were viable candidates for neuronal codes. Cluster analysis revealed that there were only a few different classes of waveforms representing each color and pattern, and there were many neurons in each class. Further, neurons that used similar waveforms to represent one color or pattern also tended to use similar waveforms to represent other colors or patterns. The waveforms representing five of the six colors and three of the six patterns were similar in the two monkeys used in this study. 4. We compared the shapes of the code waveforms across cortical areas and found no differences among areas in the shapes of the waveforms representing four of the six colors. In contrast, we found that there were differences among areas in the shapes of the waveforms representing all six patterns. These results suggest that messages about color are encoded at an early level and are then propagated upward, but that messages about pattern are altered in each successive cortical area. 5. Our results offer a neurophysiological explanation for the psychophysical evidence that color and form are processed by different channels. We propose that the psychophysical channels for color and pattern arise from the separability of the temporal codes for color and pattern in the responses of single neurons. This hypothesis implies that psychophysical channels correspond to classes of temporal codes rather than to classes of neurons.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document