Attention Limitations in the Detection and Identification of Alarms in Close Temporal Proximity

Author(s):  
Yuzhi Wan ◽  
Nadine Sarter

Objective The aim of this study was to establish the effects of simultaneous and asynchronous masking on the detection and identification of visual and auditory alarms in close temporal proximity. Background In complex and highly coupled systems, malfunctions can trigger numerous alarms within a short period of time. During such alarm floods, operators may fail to detect and identify alarms due to asynchronous and simultaneous masking. To date, the effects of masking on detection and identification have been studied almost exclusively for two alarms during single-task performance. This research examines 1) how masking affects alarm detection and identification in multitask environments and 2) whether those effects increase as a function of the number of alarms. Method Two experiments were conducted using a simulation of a drone-based package delivery service. Participants were required to ensure package delivery and respond to visual and auditory alarms associated with eight drones. The alarms were presented at various stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). The dependent measures included alarm detection rate, identification accuracy, and response time. Results Masking was observed intramodally and cross-modally for visual and auditory alarms. The SOAs at which asynchronous masking occurred were longer than reported in basic research on masking. The effects of asynchronous and, even more so, simultaneous masking became stronger as the number of alarms increased. Conclusion Masking can lead to breakdowns in the detection and identification of alarms in close temporal proximity in complex data-rich domains. Application The findings from this research provide guidance for the design of alarm systems.

Author(s):  
Yuzhi Wan ◽  
Nadine Sarter

In many complex data-rich domains, safety is highly dependent on the timely and reliable detection and identification of alarms. However, due to the coupling and complexity of systems in these environments, large numbers of alarms can occur within a short period of time – a problem called an alarm flood (Perrow, 2011). Alarm floods have been defined as more than 10 alarms in a 10-minute period (EEMUA, 1999); however, this rate is often exceeded which can lead to operators missing or misinterpreting critical alarms and, as a result, system failures and accidents. Various types of masking effects may account for observed failures to detect and identify alarms during an alarm flood. Masking occurs when one stimulus is obscured by the presence of another stimulus that appears either simultaneously or in close temporal proximity (Enns & Di Lollo V, 2000). One example of masking is an attentional blink, where the second of two stimuli is missed when presented in close temporal proximity to a preceding stimulus (Raymond, Shapiro, & Arnell, 1992). To date, attentional blinks have been studied almost exclusively in the context of two target stimuli of very short duration (less than 100ms) and in simple single-task conditions. These experiments suggest that the phenomenon occurs when two stimuli are separated by 200-600ms. However, there is limited empirical evidence (e.g., Ferris et al., 2006) that, in more complex and demanding task environments, detection performance suffers even with a longer stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). To better predict and prevent the occurrence of attentional blinks in alarm floods, the current study aimed to establish the SOA range that results in missed signals in the context of multiple visual and auditory alarms in a multi-task environment. The participants in this study were 26 students from the University of Michigan (age: 20-30 years old). The experiment was conducted using a simulation of an automated package delivery system. Participants were required to monitor the performance of eight delivery drones and perform two tasks: (1) search and confirm that a delivery pad was present before agreeing to package delivery; (2) detect and respond to visual alarms and auditory alarms associated with the various drones. Visual alarms took the form of a number presented in the center of the screen that identified the affected drone; auditory alarms used synthesized speech to present the drone number. Participants had to acknowledge the alarm as quickly as possible by pressing a button adjacent to the drone window. Both visual and auditory alarms lasted 200ms. Crossmodal matching was performed to ensure that the perceived intensity of signals in the two modalities was the same for each individual (see Pitts, Riggs, & Sarter, 2016). Alarms appeared either by themselves (single alarms) or in close temporal proximity of another alarm (alarm pairs). Each experiment scenario was 30 minutes long and included 40 single alarms and 40 alarm pairs. In addition, a 3-minute alarm flood was included in each scenario, consisting of 30 single alarms and 30 alarm pairs. The experiment employed a 5×4 full factorial design. The two independent variables, both varied within subjects, were SOA (200, 600, 800, 1000, 1200ms) and modality pairs (all four combinations of visual and auditory alarms). The dependent measures in this study were detection rate, accuracy of identification, and response time. The detection rate for visual alarms was lower when the alarm was the second in an alarm pair, compared to single visual alarms (89.9% vs. 93.9%; X2 (2, N = 22) = 6.874, p < .01). This effect was independent of the modality of the first alarm and strongest with an SOA of 1000ms. No difference was observed for the detection of single versus paired auditory alarms. Identification accuracy for visual alarms was also significantly lower when the alarm appeared second in a pair, compared to single visual alarms (86.0% vs. 94.0%; X2 (2, N = 22) = 6.007, p = .05). This effect was also independent of the modality of the first alarm, but found only with SOAs of 600, 1000, or 1200ms. Also, no significant difference in accuracy was found for single versus paired auditory alarms. Finally, response times were significantly faster during alarm floods, compared to single alarms or alarm pairs (2160ms vs. 2318ms; F (1, 21) = 6.284, p = .001). Response times to visual and auditory alarms did not differ significantly during alarm floods. In summary, in this experiment, alarm detection and identification suffered when a visual (but not an auditory) alarm was preceded by another visual or auditory alarm. This performance decrement was observed at longer SOAs than reported in earlier single-task studies. This finding may be explained, in part, by the competing visual (but not auditory) demands imposed by the required response to the alarms. Performance during alarm floods was comparable, and even improved in terms of response times, compared to single alarms and alarm pairs. This finding may be explained by the Yerkes-Dodson Law (1908) which describes that performance improves with physiological or mental arousal, up to a point, and then decreases again when arousal increases further. Another possible explanation is that participants invested more effort during alarm floods. The findings from this study add to the knowledge base in attention and alarm design. They highlight the importance of examining attentional phenomena in applied settings to be able to predict and counter performance breakdowns that may be experienced by operators engaged in multitasking in complex data-rich environments.


2016 ◽  
Vol 91 (6) ◽  
pp. 1725-1750 ◽  
Author(s):  
Marcus P. Kirk ◽  
Stanimir Markov

ABSTRACT Our study introduces analyst/investor days, a new disclosure medium that allows for private interactions with influential market participants. We also highlight interdependencies in the choice and information content of analyst/investor days and conference presentations, a well-researched disclosure medium that similarly allows for private interactions. Analyst/investor days are less frequent, but with longer duration and greater price impact than conference presentations. They are mostly hosted by firms that already have opportunities to interact with investors at conferences, but whose complex and diverse activities make the short duration and rigid format of a conference presentation an imperfect solution to these firms' information problems. Analyst/investor days and conference presentations tend to occur in different quarters, consistent with their competing for the time and attention of senior management. When these two mediums are scheduled in close temporal proximity to each other, analyst/investor days diminish the information content of conference presentations, but not vice versa, consistent with managers' favoring analyst/investor days over conference presentations as a disclosure medium. JEL Classifications: D82; M41; G11; G12; G14. Data Availability: Data are publicly available from the sources identified in the paper.


2021 ◽  
Vol 11 (40) ◽  
pp. 168-169
Author(s):  
Denise Lessa Aleixo ◽  
Leoni Vilano Bonamin ◽  
Silvana Marques De Araujo

Introduction: The use of homeopathic medicines has increased, once traditional medicines sometimes do not produce the desired effects and because side effects sometimes compromise the treatment. In recent years, research on homeopathy has clearly developed, both in the implementation of more consistent methodologies and in the description of the data and published methods, improvement are still required in these matters. The acknowledgment of homeopathy depends on the credibility of the groups researching this topic Objective: list and criticize articles highlighting main effects, schedule of treatment and potencies used in different animals models. Material and Methods: A review of articles published since 2000 in journals indexed in the PubMed/Scielo databases was performed. Keywords used were parasitosis/homeopathy and parasitosis/ultra-diluted, in English and Portuguese. Specialized journals such as Homeopathy, International Journal of High Dilution Research, and Brazilian Homeopathic Journal were also used. The contents of each issue of these journals were examined for the "Use of highly diluted medication in parasitic infections." Results and Discussion: Thirty nine papers have been gathered. The methodology of the articles surveyed did not meet the requirements listed in the REHBaR[1]. Thirty seven reports have shown the benefits/effects of highly diluted medicine in the treatment of infectious diseases. In models where experimental conditions are carefully controlled, the conclusions follow the same pattern as those observed in the treatment of farm animals, where, even without completely controlled conditions, clinical result is positive. In fourteen reports using the same model, eight where animals were treated in a constant and prolonged way shown a better result, compared with six reports in which animals were treated for a short period of time, receiving a single daily dose. Several authors have conducted clinical trials using commercial formulas, which do not always provide their composition and/or dynamization, making it difficult to reproducing the experiment. In some of the articles, it was not mentioned if the experiments were repeated at least twice. Conclusions: In parasitic infections, the effect of homeopathic medications is still controversial, and the experimental parameters for evaluation shoud be carefully chosen to avoid isolated analyses of data. Researchers should consider results regarding environmental and sanitary conditions of the animal as a whole. The improvement of techniques and expansion of knowledge about highly diluted medicines may lead to a viable alternative to treat parasitic infections. Precise and detailed descriptions will contribute to advances in the use of homeopathy, so that the wider community can benefit, in practice, from these findings. Keywords: homeopathy, parasitic infections, CAM (Complementary and Alternative Medicine). Reference [1] Lüdtke R, Musial F, Wijk R, Witt C, Baumgartner S. Reporting experiments in homeopathic basic research (REHBaR) – A detailed guideline for authors. Homeopathy. 2009; 98:287-298.


Micromachines ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 11 (11) ◽  
pp. 982
Author(s):  
Takahito Ohshiro ◽  
Yuki Komoto ◽  
Masateru Taniguchi

We utilized electrophoresis to control the fluidity of sample biomolecules in sample aqueous solutions inside the nanochannel for single-molecule detection by using a nanochannel-integrated nanogap electrode, which is composed of a nano-gap sensing electrode, nanochannel, and tapered focusing channel. In order to suppress electro-osmotic flow and thermal convection inside this nanochannel, we optimized the reduction ratios of the tapered focusing channel, and the ratio of inlet 10 μm to outlet 0.5 μm was found to be high performance of electrophoresis with lower concentration of 0.05 × TBE (Tris/Borate/EDTA) buffer containing a surfactant of 0.1 w/v% polyvinylpyrrolidone (PVP). Under the optimized conditions, single-molecule electrical measurement of deoxyguanosine monophosphate (dGMP) was performed and it was found that the throughput was significantly improved by nearly an order of magnitude compared to that without electrophoresis. In addition, it was also found that the long-duration signals that could interfere with discrimination were significantly reduced. This is because the strong electrophoresis flow inside the nanochannels prevents the molecules’ adsorption near the electrodes. This single-molecule electrical measurement with nanochannel-integrated nano-gap electrodes by electrophoresis significantly improved the throughput of signal detection and identification accuracy.


Author(s):  
D.W. Baxter ◽  
J.G. Stratford

Neurology and neurosurgery are among the most active disciplines at the Montreal General Hospital (MGH) today with impressive academic and neuroscientific profiles. This paper records an earlier period of activity when the feasibility of such research and clinical developments was only a dream.The history of neurology and neurosurgery at the MGH dates from the early days of this century – a story which is well-told by Preston Robb in “The Development of Neurology at McGill”. The level of clinical activities varied from decade to decade and from the 1930s was closely linked to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI). An MGH Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery was established in the 1940s. Francis McNaughton was the first director and, on his move to become neurologist-in-chief at the MNI in 1951, he was succeeded by Harold Elliott, the neurosurgeon. Preston Robb was then the senior neurologist, assisted over variable periods of time by colleagues Norman Viner, Miller Fisher, William Tatlow, Bernard Graham, and David Howell. Dr. Robb reluctantly resigned in 1953 after having “met with the authorities to see if a basic research program could be developed. I was told that this was not possible, it was not in the tradition of the hospital, and research was the responsibility of the university.” For a short period in 1955 and 1956, JGS was a junior staff member in neurosurgery before joining Bill Feindel at the University of Saskatchewan. Despite these impressive hospital rosters, neurologists and neurosurgeons at the MGH were not full-time and the bulk of the academic and training activities of the McGill Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery continued at the MNI.


1979 ◽  
Vol 6 (3) ◽  
pp. 411-421 ◽  
Author(s):  
George Branigan

ABSTRACTTwo suprasegmental characteristics of three children's single-word, successive single-word and multiple-word utterances were examined spectrographically. The location of terminal pitch contour and the duration of words were compared across the three utterance types. It was found that successive single-word utterances shared suprasegmental patterns, on these dimensions, with multiple-word forms but not with single words. It is argued that successive single-word utterances are not simply single words uttered in close temporal proximity. Further, an account of the underlying organization of successive utterances is proposed.


2017 ◽  
Vol 29 (9) ◽  
pp. 1621-1631 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mika Koivisto ◽  
Simone Grassini ◽  
Niina Salminen-Vaparanta ◽  
Antti Revonsuo

Detecting the presence of an object is a different process than identifying the object as a particular object. This difference has not been taken into account in designing experiments on the neural correlates of consciousness. We compared the electrophysiological correlates of conscious detection and identification directly by measuring ERPs while participants performed either a task only requiring the conscious detection of the stimulus or a higher-level task requiring its conscious identification. Behavioral results showed that, even if the stimulus was consciously detected, it was not necessarily identified. A posterior electrophysiological signature 200–300 msec after stimulus onset was sensitive for conscious detection but not for conscious identification, which correlated with a later widespread activity. Thus, we found behavioral and neural evidence for elementary visual experiences, which are not yet enriched with higher-level knowledge. The search for the mechanisms of consciousness should focus on the early elementary phenomenal experiences to avoid the confounding effects of higher-level processes.


MRS Bulletin ◽  
1987 ◽  
Vol 12 (7) ◽  
pp. 91-94
Author(s):  
Richard L. Pober ◽  
Elizabeth A. Thomson

In a relatively short period of time ceramics have become the key elements to a variety of new technologies, including integrated circuit substrates, artificial limbs, turbocharger rotors, and, of course, superconductors. By and large, however, they have not met their potential. Though advances in basic research are responsible for the breakthroughs so far, no extensive work has been done to establish the manufacturing paradigms necessary for the production of reliable, reproducible materials.The new Ceramics Manufacturing and Process Integration Laboratory (CMPIL) at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology was conceived to address this need. As a logical extension of the science-based Ceramics Processing Research Laboratory (CPRL), also at MIT, the CMPIL will “test” fundamental ideas as they relate to ceramics manufacturing. The goal is to create a hands-on “research factory,” complete with manufactured product, to make an impact on manufacturing productivity and teach students, staff, and visiting scientists the principles that control ceramics manufacturing systems. Other thrusts include developing innovative processing techniques and collecting operating data that will ultimately be transferred to industry.


2015 ◽  
pp. 453
Author(s):  
Jessica Rett ◽  
Sarah Murray

Many if not all evidential languages have a mirative evidential: an indirect evidential that can, in some contexts, mark mirativity (the expression of speaker surprise) instead of indirect evidence. We address several questions posed by this systematic polysemy: What is the affinity between indirect evidence and speaker surprise? What conditions the two interpretations? And how do mirative evidentials relate to other mirative markers? We propose a unified analysis of mirative evidentials where indirect evidentiality and mirativity involve a common epistemic component. A mirative interpretation requires a close temporal proximity between the speech event and the event of the speaker's learning the at-issue content.


1981 ◽  
Vol 71 (3) ◽  
pp. 883-903
Author(s):  
Charles A. Langston

abstract An attempt was made to model strong motion velocities and displacements from the 1965 Puget Sound earthquake. Teleseismic P waves recorded at Tumwater, Washington, were also examined to place constraints on allowable interface contrasts and to determine whether lateral heterogeneity is a major factor affecting wave propagation. Although strong motion models qualitatively showed many of the characteristics of near-vertical wave propagation in layered structures, the amplitude behavior of individual stations was quite complex. Data from Tacoma and Seattle sites attained lower velocities and acceleration compared to Olympia. The amplitude behavior is consistent with higher attenuation under Tacoma and Seattle although this is not strictly required. The short-period P data recorded at Tumwater show evidence of large velocity contrast interfaces under the station consistent with those assumed in the crustal models. The teleseismic data also indicated that dipping structure or other lateral heterogeneity is important for Olympia structure. Irrespective of these wave propagation problems, the largest single factor which has affected the level of strong ground motions in Puget Sound is the large source depth of past earthquakes. Thus, estimates of seismic hazard based on a direct interpretation of the strong motion data of the 1965 and 1949 events will be erroneously biased toward less hazard if there is potential for shallow faulting in the Puget depression.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document