Group Modeling in Social Learning Environments

2012 ◽  
Vol 10 (2) ◽  
pp. 39-56 ◽  
Author(s):  
Slavomir Stankov ◽  
Vlado Glavinic ◽  
Divna Krpan

Students’ collaboration while learning could provide better learning environments. Collaboration assumes social interactions which occur in student groups. Social theories emphasize positive influence of such interactions on learning. In order to create an appropriate learning environment that enables social interactions, it is important to carefully choose group members. In this paper, a group modeling process designed both to help students learn better and to face problems in introductory programming courses together is described. The group model is supported by graph theory and used in a popular social network environment. The authors conducted action research, analyzed students’ activities and the social environment, all of which provided us with helpful experiences for further research.

1997 ◽  
Vol 9 (2) ◽  
pp. 421-452 ◽  
Author(s):  
JOHN MORDECHAI GOTTMAN ◽  
MICHAEL J. GURALNICK ◽  
BEVERLY WILSON ◽  
CATHERINE C. SWANSON ◽  
JAMES D. MURRAY

This paper questions the assumption that children's social and emotional competence be placed within the developing child, rather than in the interaction of the child with the range of peer social ecologies in which the children might function. This paper presents a new nonstatistical mathematical approach to modeling children's peer social interaction in small groups using nonlinear difference equations in which both an uninfluenced and an influenced regulatory set point of positive minus negative interaction can be separately estimated. Using this model and the estimation procedure, it is possible to estimate what a focal child and the group initially brings to the group interaction and also how these regulatory set points are influenced by the interaction to determine two influenced regulatory set points. Six-person mainstreamed and specialized groups were established involving three types of unacquainted preschool boys: children with and without developmental delays and a language disordered but intellectually normally functioning group, using a methodology that ensured appropriate matching of child and family characteristics. For each 2-week play group, the social interactions of each child were observed during a designated free play period. Handicapped children were observed in either a specialized or mainstreamed setting. The application made of this modeling process in this paper is generating theory to attempt to understand influence processes. Parameters are introduced that reflect uninfluenced target child and group set points, emotional inertia, and influence functions.


2014 ◽  
Vol 9 (2) ◽  
pp. 204-224 ◽  
Author(s):  
Françoise Bartiaux ◽  
Luis Reátegui Salmón

Based on empirical data on “green” practices according to household size, this article questions the role, if any, given to close personal relationships by social practice theories in sustaining or not daily life practices. Data are mainly drawn from an Internet survey conducted in Belgium in 2006 by WWF-Belgium on daily practices, related to food, energy consumption, mobility, and tourism. Results show that smaller households carry out more numerous “green” practices than larger ones. The concluding discussion underlines the relevance of including social interactions—namely within the household—into the conceptual framework derived from the social theories of practices, to take into account the rearticulating role of social interactions and domestic power claims when carrying out a practice or a set of practices, and when changing it.


Behaviour ◽  
1996 ◽  
Vol 133 (11-12) ◽  
pp. 827-845 ◽  
Author(s):  
A.H. Harcourt ◽  
K.J. Stewart

We investigate the social significance of the within group ('close') calls of gorillas by examining correlates of calling with dominance rank and with relatedness of adults, and by examining whether the outcomes of interactions between adults differ depending on the calls given during the interactions. In two wild gorilla groups, the majority of adults give most of their calls when near another adult and fully in sight of them. Thus gorillas' use 'close' calls as more than mere contact calls. An adult gorilla's use of 'close' calls correlates with its own and with its partner's dominance rank, with effects being most obvious for the most dominant animals, the fully adult males. Thus the proportion of 'double grunts', the most common 'close' call, in an individual's repertoire correlated consistently with dominance rank; all non-silverback adults gave a higher percent of double grunts in the presence of subordinates than they did near dominant animals; individuals were most likely to give 'non-syllabled' grunts in the adult male's presence; and they consistently exchanged calls at a higher rate with the adult males than with other group members. An adult's type of calls did not obviously differ depending on whether their neighbour was kin or non-kin, but kin were overall more likely to give calls in the presence of kin, and to exchange calls with kin than with non-kin, although the association was not consistent throughout the two years of the study. With regard to the consequences of calling, subordinates were less likely to be feeding one minute after an approach by a fully adult male during which calls were exchanged than during silent approaches. In contrast to findings from studies of some other species, calling did not correlate with duration of grooming. We suggest that, among other functions, gorillas' 'close' calls mediate social interactions. One form of calls, 'non-syllabled' calls, are interpretable as appeasement signals. The broadest interpretation of the 'double grunt' is that it is an exaggerated announcement of presence, whose function is to attract attention to the caller, and to signal conditional future activity.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
◽  
Fiona Walls

<p>This thesis presents the findings of a project that explored the ways in which primary school children developed understandings about mathematics, mathematical 'learning' and 'knowing' and themselves as learners of mathematics. The research aimed to describe the children's mathematical learning environments, to explore the ways in which children made meaning about mathematics through social interactions within these environments, and to identify elements of these environments that appeared to enhance or inhibit the children's learning of mathematics. Located within the body of literature that takes a sociocultural view of teaching and learning, the study adopted the theoretical framework of symbolic interactionism because of its usefulness in explaining how, through the social interactions of everyday life, an individual constructs and reconstructs personal versions of 'reality', including a sense of identity. Through this lens, familiar objects, routine events and everyday language surrounding the teaching and learning of mathematics were examined for their significance to young learners. The concept of the sociomathematical world was created and developed to describe the mathematical environment of the child as positioned within wider social networks. The sociomathematical world of the child was seen as the world of everyday life, the arena in which the child, through regular and routine interactions with others, negotiated meanings about, and made personal sense of, mathematics. The research focussed on ten case study children - four girls and six boys - all attending different schools, and selected randomly from the primary schools in the Wellington region of New Zealand. For three years, from the beginning of their third year at school to the end of their fifth, the children were regularly interviewed and observed in their classrooms. Other key participants in their sociomathematical worlds were also interviewed, including families, teachers, principals, and classmates. Evidence of teaching and learning was also gathered from children's books and assessment records, and linked to local and global curriculum documentation. A cumulative picture was compiled of the mathematical teaching and learning environments of these ten children. Originally intended to be presented as separate biographies, the data were instead collated and reported according to the four distinctive recurring themes that emerged from the findings: the emphasis of speed in mathematics teaching and learning; identification and differentiation based on socially constructed perceptions of mathematical 'ability'; the establishment of 'doing maths' as solo written work; the presentation of mathematics as consisting of 'correct' and non-negotiable facts and procedures. These dominant approaches to teaching and learning of mathematics were found to conform to deeply entrenched traditions, in which the learner was viewed as the passive recipient of, rather than an active participant in, education in general and mathematics education in particular. It was found that these taken-for-granted pedagogical cultures were not explicitly supported by the official curriculum. Marked negative effects of these common teaching practices were commonly observed: alienation, marginalisation and impoverished learning. These impacts were experienced in varying forms and at varying times, by all the case study children, suggesting that changed views of mathematics and of mathematical teaching and learning are needed if the learning potential of all children is to be fully realised.</p>


Behaviour ◽  
1994 ◽  
Vol 131 (1-2) ◽  
pp. 97-113 ◽  
Author(s):  
Dario Maestripieri

AbstractThis study compared social interactions between mothers, infants, and other group members in rhesus, pigtail, and stumptail macaques living in large captive social groups. Mother-infant pairs were focally observed in 4 weekly 30-min sessions for the first 12 weeks of infant life. Rhesus and pigtail mothers were remarkably similar in several contact, proximity, and grooming measures, but their scores were lower than those of stumptail mothers. The three species did not differ quantitatively in interest shown in infants by other group members, as measured by infant handling and grooming. Infant handling in stumptail macaques was always gentle and infants were carefully avoided by other group members when off their mothers. Infant handling in rhesus and pigtail macaques also involved harassment and kidnapping. The frequency of infant harassment did not differ in rhesus and pigtail macaques but harassment was more severe in the former than in the latter species. Rhesus mothers reacted aggressively to a higher proportion of infant handling attempts than pigtail and stumptail mothers. These results confirm the hypotheses that female interest in infants does not differ among macaque species and that the quality of infant handling is a good predictor of interspecies differences in maternal protectiveness. Mothering style, however, is probably multidimensionally determined, and to fully understand interspecies differences in mother-infant relationships and their functional significance, we need to understand the mechanisms by which reproductive and ecological variables influence maternal behavior and infant development in primates.


Behaviour ◽  
1997 ◽  
Vol 134 (15-16) ◽  
pp. 1189-1210 ◽  
Author(s):  
K.A. Bard ◽  
L. Brent ◽  
J. Blangero ◽  
C.A. Bramblett ◽  
M.A. Bloomsmith

AbstractThe presence or absence of older siblings influenced the social interactions of 17 wild infant chimpanzees between the ages of 6 and 24 months living at the Gombe National Park, Tanzania. The total amount of time spent in social interaction (contact, grooming and play) was similar for infants with and without siblings, as was the overall level of social interaction with the mother. However, subjects with siblings spent more time with their siblings, while subjects with no siblings interacted with other group members. These results indicate a possible set-point for infant chimpanzee social interaction. Developmental changes were also similar in both groups, but the infants with siblings had lower levels of interaction with the mother and higher levels of interaction with other group members during the age period of 12-18 months. The levels of social behaviors were not related to the sex of the infant.


Behaviour ◽  
1988 ◽  
Vol 107 (3-4) ◽  
pp. 241-266 ◽  
Author(s):  
Eduard Stammbach

The aim of this study was to investigate the capability of monkeys to assess special characteristics in conspecifics. In a first phase I ascertained that all members of a colony of longtailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) were able to attain food by manipulating a one lever apparatus, thus introducing the "tradition" of lever pulling. Then, experiments were carried out on subgroups of the colony where only one of the lower ranking subgroup members was trained to succeed in a more complex task where three levers had to be pulled in a correct sequence. Eight specialists were established in sequence. These specialists became food producers for themselves and for the other group members. Each trial of a specialist's series was carried out in two phases. In the first, the food phase, the food dispensing apparatus was active and responses of other subgroup members to the food producing specialist were observed. In the second, the social phase, the apparatus remained inactive and observations focused on social interactions of the subgroup. As expected, primarily high ranking subgroup members attempted to participate in the food rewards gained by the specialist. It is shown that high ranking animals began to hold back their initial chasing of the specialist from the food site in course of the trials and were soon tolerated to sit near the subordinate food producer. Furthermore, some of the non-specialists began to follow or even to pass the specialist when he was approaching the apparatus to manipulate the levers. These non-specialists thus indicated that they were able to anticipate later actions. In seven out of 55 specialist-non-specialist relationships all predicted changes in social interactions occurred. In the majority of the dyads in which a change in social affiliation was registered an increase of grooming or spatial proximity was positively correlated with the amount of benefit gained from the specialist. In the social phase of the trials the non-specialists gave more grooming to the food producers and maintained spatial proximity even in this second phase. To conclude: At least some of the group members became aware of the skills of the specialists and adapted their behaviour accordingly as if to maximize benefits from their skills. Previous studies had already suggested that monkeys know about social position, social relationships and kinship of group members. This study adds a new aspect of knowledge, namely knowledge on capabilities and skills of others. Differential knowledge allows monkeys to select partners optimally according to their skills and social position.


1993 ◽  
Vol 71 (10) ◽  
pp. 1945-1953 ◽  
Author(s):  
Catherine Perrin ◽  
Jacques Coulon ◽  
Michel Le Berre

The composition and social interactions of three family groups of Marmota marmota were observed during a study in the Vanoise National Park (French Alps) from mid-June to late September. Interactions between members of the same group were mainly cohesive, which confirms the social nature of this species. Social interactions between marmots from different family groups were rare and agonistic. The rate of social interactions decreased as the season progressed, probably in relation to a shift in allocation of energy to preparation for hibernation. Some cohesive interactions increased just before immergence, when marmots restricted their activity to the main burrow system. Rates of social interactions differed between groups and between individuals. Fewer agonistic acts, more play-fighting, and persistence of play-fighting late in the active season among immatures suggest the influence of an ontogenetic component. The most agonistic of the studied groups included an adult male that had recently immigrated. He focused most of his agonistic acts on three 2-year-old males, of which two dispersed during the season. Social events characteristic of each group, individual variability, and level of familiarity between group members influence the quality of social interactions and the dispersal of immatures. Social relationships between familiar individuals are amicable and can facilitate the retention of immatures in their natal group as well as their recruitment by emigrant adults.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Larissa Lauren Meijer ◽  
Birgit Hasenack ◽  
Jonas Kamps ◽  
Andy Mahon ◽  
Guilia Titone ◽  
...  

Interpersonal touch and affective touch play a crucial role in social interactions and have a positive influence on mental health. The social distancing regulations related to the COVID-19 pandemic have reduced the ability to engage in interpersonal touch. This could cause touch deprivation, and it might alter the way in which affective touch is perceived. To investigate this, we conducted an online survey with 2348 participants, which contained questions regarding the COVID-19 regulations, touch deprivation and the perceived pleasantness of affective and non-affective touch. Results showed that participants reported feelings of touch deprivation. This significantly increased with the duration and severity of the COVID-19 regulations. Participants who experienced more touch deprivation rated videos of affective and non-affective touch as more pleasant. Current results provide insight in the impact of sudden and prolonged COVID-19 regulations and show that increasing the duration and severity of these regulations is associated with a higher desire for touch, which leads to increased perceived pleasantness of touch.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
◽  
Fiona Walls

<p>This thesis presents the findings of a project that explored the ways in which primary school children developed understandings about mathematics, mathematical 'learning' and 'knowing' and themselves as learners of mathematics. The research aimed to describe the children's mathematical learning environments, to explore the ways in which children made meaning about mathematics through social interactions within these environments, and to identify elements of these environments that appeared to enhance or inhibit the children's learning of mathematics. Located within the body of literature that takes a sociocultural view of teaching and learning, the study adopted the theoretical framework of symbolic interactionism because of its usefulness in explaining how, through the social interactions of everyday life, an individual constructs and reconstructs personal versions of 'reality', including a sense of identity. Through this lens, familiar objects, routine events and everyday language surrounding the teaching and learning of mathematics were examined for their significance to young learners. The concept of the sociomathematical world was created and developed to describe the mathematical environment of the child as positioned within wider social networks. The sociomathematical world of the child was seen as the world of everyday life, the arena in which the child, through regular and routine interactions with others, negotiated meanings about, and made personal sense of, mathematics. The research focussed on ten case study children - four girls and six boys - all attending different schools, and selected randomly from the primary schools in the Wellington region of New Zealand. For three years, from the beginning of their third year at school to the end of their fifth, the children were regularly interviewed and observed in their classrooms. Other key participants in their sociomathematical worlds were also interviewed, including families, teachers, principals, and classmates. Evidence of teaching and learning was also gathered from children's books and assessment records, and linked to local and global curriculum documentation. A cumulative picture was compiled of the mathematical teaching and learning environments of these ten children. Originally intended to be presented as separate biographies, the data were instead collated and reported according to the four distinctive recurring themes that emerged from the findings: the emphasis of speed in mathematics teaching and learning; identification and differentiation based on socially constructed perceptions of mathematical 'ability'; the establishment of 'doing maths' as solo written work; the presentation of mathematics as consisting of 'correct' and non-negotiable facts and procedures. These dominant approaches to teaching and learning of mathematics were found to conform to deeply entrenched traditions, in which the learner was viewed as the passive recipient of, rather than an active participant in, education in general and mathematics education in particular. It was found that these taken-for-granted pedagogical cultures were not explicitly supported by the official curriculum. Marked negative effects of these common teaching practices were commonly observed: alienation, marginalisation and impoverished learning. These impacts were experienced in varying forms and at varying times, by all the case study children, suggesting that changed views of mathematics and of mathematical teaching and learning are needed if the learning potential of all children is to be fully realised.</p>


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document