social phenotype
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

30
(FIVE YEARS 9)

H-INDEX

12
(FIVE YEARS 2)

2022 ◽  
Vol 13 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Meredith Cola ◽  
Lisa D. Yankowitz ◽  
Kimberly Tena ◽  
Alison Russell ◽  
Leila Bateman ◽  
...  

Abstract Background Autistic individuals frequently experience social communication challenges. Girls are diagnosed with autism less often than boys even when their symptoms are equally severe, which may be due to insufficient understanding of the way autism manifests in girls. Differences in the behavioral presentation of autism, including how people talk about social topics, could contribute to these persistent problems with identification. Despite a growing body of research suggesting that autistic girls and boys present distinct symptom profiles in a variety of domains, including social attention, friendships, social motivation, and language, differences in the way that autistic boys and girls communicate verbally are not yet well understood. Closely analyzing boys’ and girls’ socially-focused language during semi-structured clinical assessments could shed light on potential sex differences in the behavioral presentation of autistic individuals that may prove useful for identifying and effectively supporting autistic girls. Here, we compare social word use in verbally fluent autistic girls and boys during the interview sections of the ADOS-2 Module 3 and measure associations with clinical phenotype. Methods School-aged girls and boys with autism (N = 101, 25 females; aged 6–15) were matched on age, IQ, and parent/clinician ratings of autism symptom severity. Our primary analysis compared the number of social words produced by autistic boys and girls (normalized to account for differences in total word production). Social words are words that make reference to other people, including friends and family. Results There was a significant main effect of sex on social word production, such that autistic girls used more social words than autistic boys. To identify the specific types of words driving this effect, additional subcategories of friend and family words were analyzed. There was a significant effect of sex on friend words, with girls using significantly more friend words than boys. However, there was no significant main effect of sex on family words, suggesting that sex differences in social word production may be driven by girls talking more about friends compared to boys, not family. To assess relationships between word use and clinical phenotype, we modeled ADOS-2 Social Affect (SA) scores as a function of social word production. In the overall sample, social word use correlated significantly with ADOS-2 SA scores, indicating that participants who used more social words were rated as less socially impaired by clinicians. However, when examined in each sex separately, this result only held for boys. Limitations This study cannot speak to the ways in which social word use may differ for younger children, adults, or individuals who are not verbally fluent; in addition, there were more autistic boys than girls in our sample, making it difficult to detect small effects. Conclusions Autistic girls used significantly more social words than boys during a diagnostic assessment—despite being matched on age, IQ, and both parent- and clinician-rated autism symptom severity. Sex differences in linguistic markers of social phenotype in autism are especially important in light of the late or missed diagnoses that disproportionately affect autistic girls. Specifically, heightened talk about social topics could complicate autism referral and diagnosis when non-clinician observers expect a male-typical pattern of reduced social focus, which autistic girls may not always exhibit.


Genetics ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 219 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Robert E Page

Abstract Understanding the organization and evolution of social complexity is a major task because it requires building an understanding of mechanisms operating at different levels of biological organization from genes to social interactions. I discuss here, a unique forward genetic approach spanning more than 30 years beginning with human-assisted colony-level selection for a single social trait, the amount of pollen honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) store. The goal was to understand a complex social trait from the social phenotype to genes responsible for observed trait variation. The approach combined the results of colony-level selection with detailed studies of individual behavior and physiology resulting in a mapped, integrated phenotypic architecture composed of correlative relationships between traits spanning anatomy, physiology, sensory response systems, and individual behavior that affect individual foraging decisions. Colony-level selection reverse engineered the architecture of an integrated phenotype of individuals resulting in changes in the social trait. Quantitative trait locus (QTL) studies combined with an exceptionally high recombination rate (60 kb/cM), and a phenotypic map, provided a genotype–phenotype map of high complexity demonstrating broad QTL pleiotropy, epistasis, and epistatic pleiotropy suggesting that gene pleiotropy or tight linkage of genes within QTL integrated the phenotype. Gene expression and knockdown of identified positional candidates revealed genes affecting foraging behavior and confirmed one pleiotropic gene, a tyramine receptor, as a target for colony-level selection that was under selection in two different tissues in two different life stages. The approach presented here has resulted in a comprehensive understanding of the structure and evolution of honey bee social organization.


2021 ◽  
pp. 004005992110220
Author(s):  
Alyssa R. Henry ◽  
Emily J. Solari

Many children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) experience associated difficulties in reading comprehension. This may be due to the social nature of fictional texts, which require the reader to interpret what characters are thinking and feeling and to make inferences about the cause and effect between events in a story. This paper outlines strategies that teachers can use to assist their learners with ASD in reading comprehension, including activating social background knowledge, encouraging series and familiar genres, identifying and teaching figurative language, and using graphic organizers to support perspective-taking. These strategies are tailored to the social phenotype of children with ASD to best address the underlying difficulties that may be impeding their reading comprehension.


2021 ◽  
Vol 4 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Wyatt A. Shell ◽  
Michael A. Steffen ◽  
Hannah K. Pare ◽  
Arun S. Seetharam ◽  
Andrew J. Severin ◽  
...  

AbstractWhile it is well known that the genome can affect social behavior, recent models posit that social lifestyles can, in turn, influence genome evolution. Here, we perform the most phylogenetically comprehensive comparative analysis of 16 bee genomes to date: incorporating two published and four new carpenter bee genomes (Apidae: Xylocopinae) for a first-ever genomic comparison with a monophyletic clade containing solitary through advanced eusocial taxa. We find that eusocial lineages have undergone more gene family expansions, feature more signatures of positive selection, and have higher counts of taxonomically restricted genes than solitary and weakly social lineages. Transcriptomic data reveal that caste-affiliated genes are deeply-conserved; gene regulatory and functional elements are more closely tied to social phenotype than phylogenetic lineage; and regulatory complexity increases steadily with social complexity. Overall, our study provides robust empirical evidence that social evolution can act as a major and surprisingly consistent driver of macroevolutionary genomic change.


Author(s):  
Amber Song ◽  
Meredith Cola ◽  
Samantha Plate ◽  
Victoria Petrulla ◽  
Lisa Yankowitz ◽  
...  

2020 ◽  
Vol 2 (7A) ◽  
Author(s):  
Lucy McCully ◽  
Sarah Seaton ◽  
Alana McGraw ◽  
Mark Silby

Bacteria often reside in multi-species communities where many behaviors result from interspecies relationships. In a two-species community, we show that co-culture of Pseudomonas fluorescens and Pedobacter sp. permits motility across a hard agar surface where neither species moves alone. Pseudomonas species engage in surface motility, including swimming and swarming, but these require moist environments. We are exploring the role of the Pseudomonas flagella in social motility. We deleted genes related to flagellar structure and function to study the importance of flagellar elements on the social phenotype. Using microscopy and swimming assays, we evaluate the effects of gene deletions on the presence and function of flagella in the resulting mutants, and evaluate the effect on social motility by observing the phenotype on both hard (2% w/v) and soft agar (1% w/v). Removal of the flagellar filament abolishes social motility, indicating a requirement for flagella in social motility. Removal of the flagellar motor also abolishes social motility, demonstrating that flagella must be functional. However, removal of membrane-spanning structural components, or part of the type III secretion system, results in mutants that lack flagella, but participate in a similar motile behavior with Pedobacter. Here we describe a role for flagella in motility of a two-species consortium across a hard agar surface, an environment considered non-permissive for flagellar motility. The requirement for both bacterial species indicates we are observing motility as a social phenotype, with a contribution from Pedobacter that enables the Pseudomonas flagella to function under conditions relevant in the natural soil environment.


2019 ◽  
Vol 101 ◽  
pp. 101727 ◽  
Author(s):  
Andrea Lenzi ◽  
Marianna Maranghi ◽  
Giovanni Stilo ◽  
Paola Velardi

Science ◽  
2019 ◽  
Vol 363 (6433) ◽  
pp. 1342-1345 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sébastien Wielgoss ◽  
Rebekka Wolfensberger ◽  
Lei Sun ◽  
Francesca Fiegna ◽  
Gregory J. Velicer

The composition of cooperative systems, including animal societies, organismal bodies, and microbial groups, reflects their past and shapes their future evolution. However, genomic diversity within many multiunit systems remains uncharacterized, limiting our ability to understand and compare their evolutionary character. We have analyzed genomic and social-phenotype variation among 120 natural isolates of the cooperative bacterium Myxococcus xanthus derived from six multicellular fruiting bodies. Each fruiting body was composed of multiple lineages radiating from a unique recent ancestor. Genomic evolution was concentrated in selection hotspots associated with evolutionary change in social phenotypes. Synonymous mutations indicated that kin lineages within the same fruiting body often first diverged from a common ancestor more than 100 generations ago. Thus, selection appears to promote endemic diversification of kin lineages that remain together over long histories of local interaction, thereby potentiating social coevolution.


2018 ◽  
Vol 14 (5) ◽  
pp. 20180150 ◽  
Author(s):  
N. C. Bennett ◽  
A. Ganswindt ◽  
S. B. Ganswindt ◽  
J. U. M. Jarvis ◽  
M. Zöttl ◽  
...  

Elevated prolactin (PRL) has been associated with the expression of social and cooperative behaviours in a number of vertebrate species, as well as suppression of reproduction. As social mole-rats exhibit both of these traits, PRL is a prime candidate in mediating their social phenotype. While naked and Damaraland mole-rats (NMRs and DMRs) have evolved eusociality independently within their family, both species exhibit an extreme skew in lifetime reproductive success, with breeding restricted to a single female and one or two males. Non-breeding NMRs of both sexes are physiologically inhibited from reproducing, while in DMRs only the non-breeding females are physiologically suppressed. Newly emerging work has implicated the dopamine system and PRL as a component in socially induced reproductive suppression and eusociality in NMR, but the DMR remains unstudied in this context. To investigate evolutionary convergence in the role of PRL in shaping African mole-rat eusociality, we determined plasma PRL concentrations in breeders and non-breeders of both sexes, comparing DMRs with NMRs. Among samples from non-breeding NMRs 80% had detectable plasma PRL concentrations. As a benchmark, these often (37%) exceeding those considered clinically hyperprolactinaemic (25 ng ml −1 ) in humans: mean ± s.e.m.: 34.81 ± 5.87 ngml −1 ; range 0.00–330.30 ng ml −1 . Conversely, 85% of non-breeding DMR samples had undetectable values and none had concentrations above 25 ng ml −1 : 0.71 ± 0.38 ng ml −1 ; 0.00–23.87 ngml −1 . Breeders in both species had the expected variance in plasma PRL concentrations as part of normal reproductive function, with lactating queens having significantly higher values. These results suggest that while elevated PRL in non-breeders is implicated in NMR eusociality, this may not be the case in DMRs, and suggests a lack of evolutionary convergence in the proximate control of the social phenotype in these mole-rats.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document