control failure
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

210
(FIVE YEARS 67)

H-INDEX

23
(FIVE YEARS 3)

2021 ◽  
Vol 11 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Sathya Narayana Sharma ◽  
Azizuddin Khan

AbstractDecision makers tend to give magnified significance to immediately available rewards which leads to intertemporal preference reversals, which is a form of self-control failure. The objective of the present study was to understand the cognitive and neural underpinnings of this phenomenon using event-related potentials (ERP) and their source localization using standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography analysis (sLORETA). Twenty-four participants performed a money choice task, where they made choices between a smaller-sooner and a larger-later reward, which included trials with and without an immediately available option, while their electroencephalography (EEG) activity was recorded. Trials with and without immediacy were identical except that the latter involved a front-end delay added to both the rewards. Results showed that presence of immediacy made the choices significantly more impulsive. Presence of immediate reward elicited larger visual P2 and late positive potential (LPP), indicating enhanced capture of automatic and sustained attention respectively, and smaller N2, indicative of diminished engagement of cognitive control processes. Source localization revealed increased activity in the visual cortex in the presence of immediacy, signifying higher valuation. Higher activation of areas of insula during P2—suggesting increased awareness of visceral signals—predicted larger impulsive preference reversals. The results suggest that presence of immediate reward biases the choice very early during the decision making process by precipitating visceral states that triggers approach behaviour, and highlight the need to adopt strategies like precommitment to counter the effect.


2021 ◽  
Vol 8 (Supplement_1) ◽  
pp. S429-S430
Author(s):  
Mary Tabatneck ◽  
Jeffrey Campbell ◽  
Mingwei Sun ◽  
Wei He ◽  
Gabriella S Lamb ◽  
...  

Abstract Background The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends use of Interferon-Gamma Release Assays (IGRAs) to diagnose tuberculosis (TB) infection in patients ≥2 years old. However, IGRAs are not currently recommended in younger patients due to limited data and concerns of invalid/indeterminate test results, which occur if there is a positive or negative control failure. We sought to characterize the patterns of IGRA use in clinical practice and results of IGRAs in patients < 2 years old. Methods We conducted a retrospective cohort study of children < 2 years old at two large health systems in the Boston area who had IGRA and/or tuberculin skin test (TST) performed from October 1, 2015 – January 31, 2021. We reviewed medical records to determine IGRA test type, IGRA result (positive, negative, invalid/indeterminate) and location of testing (outpatient primary care, outpatient subspecialty, inpatient). We summarized test interpretability, location, and changes in proportion of IGRA vs. TST. Results We identified 330 IGRA (268 T-SPOT.TB, 62 QuantiFERON Gold) and 2029 TST results among 1982 patients who were < 2 years old (range: 11 days – 1.9 years). Monthly proportion of IGRAs among all TB tests ordered increased from 2015 to 2021 (Figure 1) (Pearson correlation coefficient 0.85, P < 0.001). Among IGRA results, 314 (95%) were negative, 3 (1%) were positive, and 13 (4%) were invalid/indeterminate (11 T-SPOT.TB, 2 QuantiFERON Gold). Of 324 IGRA tests for which testing location was known, 233 (72%) and 91 (28%) were ordered in outpatient and inpatient settings, respectively. Of tests in outpatient settings, 132 (57%) were ordered in primary care offices, 53 (23%) were ordered in subspecialist offices, and 48 (21%) were obtained in outpatient labs of unidentified clinics. Tuberculosis infection tests and proportion IGRA. Total number of tests and proportion of IGRA:TST obtained by month, from October 2015-January 2021. Conclusion While most TB infection tests in this age group were TSTs, the monthly proportion of tests that were IGRAs increased over time between 2015-2021. IGRAs were obtained in varied clinical settings. In this low-burden setting, rates of invalid/indeterminate IGRAs were low among children < 2 years old, which suggests that IGRAs are reasonable TB testing options for patients < 2 years old, and may be preferred given limitations of TSTs. Disclosures Gabriella S. Lamb, MD, MPH, Nothing to disclose


Genealogy ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 5 (4) ◽  
pp. 93
Author(s):  
Mike Slaven

The “everyday bordering” concept has provided key insights into the effects of diverse bordering practices upon social life, placing the bordering of the welfare state among wider state interventions in an autochthonous politics of belonging. Sociological contributions have also introduced new explanations as to why states pursue such measures, positing that neoliberal states seek legitimacy through increasing activities to (re)affirm borders within this politics of belonging, compensating for a failure to govern the economy in the interests of citizens. To what extent is this visible in the state-led emergence of (everyday) borders around welfare in the United Kingdom, often cited as a key national case? This article draws from 20 elite interviews to contribute to genealogical accounts of the emergence of everyday bordering through identifying the developing “problematizations” connected to this kind of bordering activity, as the British state began to distinctly involve welfare-state actors in bordering policies in the 1990s and early 2000s. This evidence underlines how these policies were tied to a “pull factor” problematization of control failure, where the state needed to reduce various “pull factors” purportedly attracting unwanted migrants in order to control immigration per se, with little evidence that legitimacy issues tied to perceived declining economic governability informed these developments in this period. These findings can inform future genealogical analyses that trace the emergence of everyday bordering.


Author(s):  
J. L. Vieira ◽  
S. O. Campos ◽  
G. Smagghe ◽  
D. B. Fragoso ◽  
J. A. F. Barrigossi ◽  
...  

PLoS ONE ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 16 (10) ◽  
pp. e0257717
Author(s):  
Christian Dirk Wiesner ◽  
Jennifer Meyer ◽  
Christoph Lindner

Self-control enables people to override momentary thoughts, emotions, or impulses in order to pursue long-term goals. Good self-control is a predictor for health, success, and subjective well-being, as bad self-control is for the opposite. Therefore, the question arises why evolution has not endowed us with perfect self-control. In this article, we draw some attention to the hidden benefits of self-control failure and present a new experimental paradigm that captures both costs and benefits of self-control failure. In an experiment, participants worked on three consecutive tasks: 1) In a transcription task, we manipulated how much effortful self-control two groups of participants had to exert. 2) In a number-comparison task, participants of both groups were asked to compare numbers and ignore distracting neutral versus reward-related pictures. 3) After a pause for recreation, participants were confronted with an unannounced recognition task measuring whether they had incidentally encoded the distracting pictures during the previous number-comparison task. The results showed that participants who exerted a high amount of effortful self-control during the first task shifted their priorities and attention toward the distractors during the second self-control demanding task: The cost of self-control failure was reflected in worse performance in the number-comparison task. Moreover, the group which had exerted a high amount of self-control during the first task and showed self-control failure during the second task was better in the unannounced third task. The benefit of self-control failure during number comparison was reflected in better performance during the recognition task. However, costs and benefits were not specific for reward-related distractors but also occurred with neutral pictures. We propose that the hidden benefit of self-control failure lies in the exploration of distractors present during goal pursuit, i.e. the collection of information about the environment and the potential discovery of new sources of reward. Detours increase local knowledge.


Author(s):  
Jorge Braz Torres ◽  
G G Rolim ◽  
D M Potin ◽  
L S Arruda ◽  
R C S Neves

Abstract Synthetic insecticide application is one tactic for reducing boll weevil, Anthonomus grandis grandis Boheman (Coleoptera: Curculionidae), infestations during the cotton, Gossypium hirsutum L., reproductive stage. We assessed the susceptibility of the boll weevil and its natural enemies to ethiprole (mode of action 2B), a phenylpyrazole insecticide, and diagnostic concentrations of ethiprole indicative of boll weevil susceptibility. Differences in the lethal concentrations of ethiprole were calculated with susceptibility ratios based on LC50 ranging from 2.89- to 10.34-fold relative to a natural susceptible population. The lowest and the highest recommended field rates of ethiprole, 100 and 200 g a.i./ha, produced residues that caused 83.3% and 93.7% mortality of weevils caged with cotton leaves from field-treated plants for 8 d. We found that ethiprole was less toxic than fipronil to the boll weevil parasitoid Bracon vulgaris Ashmead (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) and to the red imported fire ant, Solenopsis invicta Buren (Hymenoptera: Formicidae), while fipronil was highly toxic to both. Adult earwigs, Euborellia annulipes Lucas (Dermaptera: Anisolabididae), were relatively tolerant to ethiprole and fipronil at the highest field rates. Pooled LC50-and LC95-concentrations of ethiprole calculated from studied populations were used as diagnostic for boll weevil mortality, and the outcome fitted to the expected mortality for boll weevil populations from different locations serving for further control failure assessment. Ethiprole appears to be suitable for boll weevil control with low impact on natural enemy communities.


2021 ◽  
Vol 118 (35) ◽  
pp. e2018726118
Author(s):  
Candace M. Raio ◽  
Paul W. Glimcher

Since Odysseus committed to resisting the Sirens, mechanisms to limit self-control failure have been a central feature of human behavior. Psychologists have long argued that the use of self-control is an effortful process and, more recently, that its failure arises when the cognitive costs of self-control outweigh its perceived benefits. In a similar way, economists have argued that sophisticated choosers can adopt “precommitment strategies” that tie the hands of their future selves in order to reduce these costs. Yet, we still lack an empirical tool to quantify and demonstrate the cost of self-control. Here, we develop and validate an economic decision-making task to quantify the subjective cost of self-control by determining the monetary cost a person is willing to incur in order to eliminate the need for self-control. We find that humans will pay to avoid having to exert self-control in a way that scales with increasing levels of temptation and that these costs appear to be modulated both by motivational incentives and stress exposure. Our psychophysical approach allows us to index moment-to-moment self-control costs at the within-subject level, validating important theoretical work across multiple disciplines and opening avenues of self-control research in healthy and clinical populations.


Author(s):  
Woochun Jun ◽  

The development of information and communication technology and smart technology in modern society makes daily life very convenient for modern people, while also causing various side effects. These side effects typically appear in various forms, including cyberbullying, Internet addiction, personal information infringement, and the distribution of harmful information. Especially with the development and distribution of smart devices, smartphone overdependence is very serious, especially among teenagers. The purpose of this study is to analyse the causes of overdependence on smartphones in Korean teenagers and also to suggest solutions. The overdependence of smartphones is represented by three phenomena: self-control failure, salience, and serious consequences. A thorough statistical analysis showed that self-control failure was the largest among the three phenomena of overdependence on smartphones, followed by salience and serious consequences. Teenager smartphone over-dependents personally try to solve smartphone overdependence through strengthening smartphone self-control capability and alternative leisure activities. However, both their main and desired leisure activities are smartphone use, and they want other alternative activities, but they cannot escape using smartphones. It is hoped that the results of this study will help follow-up research in the future along with policy directions on teenagers’ overdependence on smartphones. Keywords—Smartphone Overdependence, Smartphone Addiction; Teenagers, Information and Communication Technology, Smart Technology, Information Ethics


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document