subjective confidence
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

105
(FIVE YEARS 40)

H-INDEX

18
(FIVE YEARS 1)

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Yusuke Haruki ◽  
Kenji Ogawa

Perception of internal bodily sensations or interoception has recently been studied under a predictive coding framework. In this framework, the brain utilizes both top-down prediction and bottom-up prediction error signals to determine the content of the perception through inferences regarding the cause of the ongoing sensation. Particularly, interoception and other exteroceptive sensory modalities are considered to share an integrated, intertwined process of inference. Thus, it is possible that exteroceptive stimuli interfere with the inference of interoception. Hence, we investigated whether auditory stimuli disrupted interoceptive inference that resulted in diminished awareness of interoception. Thirty healthy volunteers performed the heartbeat counting task with and without distractor sounds. The psychophysiological traits that would reflect the individual differences in prior prediction signals of interoception were measured as the high-frequency component of the heart rate variability (HF-HRV) at rest and trait interoceptive sensibility. The results showed that the auditory distractor diminished objective interoceptive accuracy, subjective confidence in interoception, and the intensity of the heartbeat, suggesting disrupted interoceptive inference under external stimuli. Importantly, individual differences in the distractor effect were modulated by both the HF-HRV and tendency to worry about bodily states. These findings support and extend the predictive coding account of interoception by suggesting that interoceptive inference could be disrupted by external stimuli and that such disruption may be modulated by a difference in prior predictions and its precision regarding interoception.


2021 ◽  
Vol 12 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Koosha Khalvati ◽  
Roozbeh Kiani ◽  
Rajesh P. N. Rao

AbstractIn perceptual decisions, subjects infer hidden states of the environment based on noisy sensory information. Here we show that both choice and its associated confidence are explained by a Bayesian framework based on partially observable Markov decision processes (POMDPs). We test our model on monkeys performing a direction-discrimination task with post-decision wagering, demonstrating that the model explains objective accuracy and predicts subjective confidence. Further, we show that the model replicates well-known discrepancies of confidence and accuracy, including the hard-easy effect, opposing effects of stimulus variability on confidence and accuracy, dependence of confidence ratings on simultaneous or sequential reports of choice and confidence, apparent difference between choice and confidence sensitivity, and seemingly disproportionate influence of choice-congruent evidence on confidence. These effects may not be signatures of sub-optimal inference or discrepant computational processes for choice and confidence. Rather, they arise in Bayesian inference with incomplete knowledge of the environment.


2021 ◽  
Vol 14 (4) ◽  
Author(s):  
Ondřej Straka ◽  
Šárka Portešová ◽  
Daniela Halámková ◽  
Michal Jabůrek

In this paper, we inquire into possible differences between children with exceptionally high intellectual abilities and their average peers as regards metacognitive monitoring and related metacognitive strategies. The question whether gifted children surpass their typically developing peers not only in the intellectual abilities, but also in their level of metacognitive skills, has not been convincingly answered so far. We sought to examine the indicators of metacognitive behavior by means of eye-tracking technology and to compare these findings with the participants’ subjective confidence ratings. Eye-movement data of gifted and average students attending final grades of primary school (4th and 5th grades) were recorded while they dealt with a deductive reasoning task, and four metrics supposed to bear on metacognitive skills, namely the overall trial duration, mean fixation duration, number of regressions and normalized gaze transition entropy, were analyzed. No significant differences between gifted and average children were found in the normalized gaze transition entropy, in mean fixation duration, nor - after controlling for the trial duration – in number of regressions. Both groups of children differed in the time devoted to solving the task. Both groups significantly differed in the association between time devoted to the task and the participants’ subjective confidence rating, where only the gifted children tended to devote more time when they felt less confident. Several implications of these findings are discussed.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lena Esther Ptasczynski ◽  
Isa Steinecker ◽  
Philipp Sterzer ◽  
Matthias Guggenmos

Reinforcement learning algorithms have a long-standing success story in explaining the dynamics of instrumental conditioning in humans and other species. While normative reinforcement learning models are critically dependent on external feedback, recent findings in the field of perceptual learning point to a crucial role of internally-generated reinforcement signals based on subjective confidence, when external feedback is not available. Here, we investigated the existence of such confidence-based learning signals in a key domain of reinforcement-based learning: instrumental conditioning. We conducted a value-based decision making experiment which included phases with and without external feedback and in which participants reported their confidence in addition to choices. Behaviorally, we found signatures of self-reinforcement in phases without feedback, reflected in an increase of subjective confidence and choice consistency. To clarify the mechanistic role of confidence in value-based learning, we compared a family of confidence-based learning models with more standard models predicting either no change in value estimates or a devaluation over time when no external reward is provided. We found that confidence-based models indeed outperformed these reference models, whereby the learning signal of the winning model was based on the prediction error between current confidence and a stimulus-unspecific average of previous confidence levels. Interestingly, individuals with more volatile reward-based value updates in the presence of feedback also showed more volatile confidence-based value updates when feedback was not available. Together, our results provide evidence that confidence-based learning signals affect instrumentally learned subjective values in the absence of external feedback.


Author(s):  
Regan M. Gallagher ◽  
Thomas Suddendorf ◽  
Derek H. Arnold

AbstractViewing static images depicting movement can result in a motion aftereffect: people tend to categorise direction signals as moving in the opposite direction relative to the implied motion in still photographs. This finding could indicate that inferred motion direction can penetrate sensory processing and change perception. Equally possible, however, is that inferred motion changes decision processes, but not perception. Here we test these two possibilities. Since both categorical decisions and subjective confidence are informed by sensory information, confidence can be informative about whether an aftereffect probably results from changes to perceptual or decision processes. We therefore used subjective confidence as an additional measure of the implied motion aftereffect. In Experiment 1 (implied motion), we find support for decision-level changes only, with no change in subjective confidence. In Experiment 2 (real motion), we find equal changes to decisions and confidence. Our results suggest the implied motion aftereffect produces a bias in decision-making, but leaves perceptual processing unchanged.


2021 ◽  
Vol 12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Alexander Handler ◽  
Sascha Frühholz

Mugbook searches are conducted in case a suspect is not known and to assess if a previously convicted person might be recognized as a potential culprit. The goal of the two experiments reported here was to analyze if prior statements and information about the suspect can aid in the evaluation if such a mugbook search is subsequently advised or not. In experiment 1, memory accuracy for person descriptors was tested in order to analyze, which attributes could be chosen to down-scale the mugbook prior to testing. Results showed that age was the most accurate descriptor, followed by ethnicity and height. At the same time self-assessed low subjective accuracy of culprit descriptions by the witness seemed to be divergent to the objective actual performance accuracy. In experiment 2, a mugbook search was conducted after participants viewed a video of a staged crime and gave a description of the culprit. Results showed that accuracy in mugbook searches correlated positively with the total number of person descriptors given by the witness as well as with witness’ description of external facial features. Predictive confidence (i.e., subjective rating of own performance in the subsequent mugbook search), however did not show any relation to the identification accuracy in the actual mugbook search. These results highlight the notion that mugbooks should not be conducted according to the subjective estimation of the witness’ performance but more according to the actual statements and descriptions that the witness can give about the culprit.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document