Stressful life events and neuroticism as predictors of late-life versus early-life depression

2013 ◽  
Vol 13 (4) ◽  
pp. 221-228 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kerstin Weber ◽  
Panteleimon Giannakopoulos ◽  
François R. Herrmann ◽  
Javier Bartolomei ◽  
Sergio DiGiorgio ◽  
...  
PLoS ONE ◽  
2010 ◽  
Vol 5 (7) ◽  
pp. e11523 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jasveer Virk ◽  
Jiong Li ◽  
Mogens Vestergaard ◽  
Carsten Obel ◽  
Michael Lu ◽  
...  

2010 ◽  
Vol 31 (5) ◽  
pp. 886-887 ◽  
Author(s):  
Tom Power ◽  
Robert Stewart ◽  
Marie-Laure Ancelin ◽  
Isabelle Jaussent ◽  
Alain Malafosse ◽  
...  

1987 ◽  
Vol 17 (4) ◽  
pp. 299-308 ◽  
Author(s):  
John A. Humphrey ◽  
Stuart Palmer

Stressful life events experienced by a 50 percent random sample of criminal homicide offenders (270) and a 30 percent random sample of nonviolent felonious property offenders (194) incarcerated in North Carolina over a two-year period are analyzed. Dohrenwend et al. Psychiatric Epidemiological Research Interview (PERI) was expanded to include occurrences in childhood and was used to assess the extent of stressful life events. These events were divided into loss and nonloss items; and whether the event occurred in the early life of the offender (past event) or in closer proximity to the crime (recent event). Murderers were divided into primary offenders, those whose victims were family members or close friends; and nonprimary offenders, those who killed strangers or mere acquaintances. The findings show that criminal homicide offenders experienced stressful life events in greater number and more consistently than did nonviolent felons. Further, primary homicide offenders tended to suffer more stressful life events involving loss, although nonprimary offenders experienced more nonloss stress events.


2001 ◽  
Vol 179 (5) ◽  
pp. 426-431 ◽  
Author(s):  
Edwin De Beurs ◽  
Aartjan Beekman ◽  
Sandra Geerlings ◽  
Dorly Deeg ◽  
Richard Van Dyck ◽  
...  

BackgroundLittle research has been done on the uniqueness of risk profiles for depression and anxiety in late life.AimsDelineating risk factors for the decline of mental health in older persons, comparing risk profiles for developing symptoms of pure depression, pure anxiety and both anxiety and depression in a prospective design.MethodSelf-Report data on depression and anxiety were collected from community-dwelling older respondents (⩾55 years) on two occasions, 3 years apart. Data from emotionally healthy respondents (n=1810) were used to investigate the effects of long-standing vulnerability factors and stressful life events.ResultsAfter 3 years 9% of the subjects had scored beyond the thresholds for symptoms. Vulnerability for depression and anxiety was quite similar, but life events differed: onset of depression was predicted by death of a partner or other relatives; onset of anxiety was best predicted by having a partner who developed a major illness. No support for moderator effects between vulnerability factors and stress was found; the effects were purely additive.ConclusionsDepression and anxiety have many risk factors in common, but specific risk factors also were found, especially in subjects developing both depression and anxiety.


2018 ◽  
Vol 35 ◽  
pp. 37-45 ◽  
Author(s):  
Michael J. McFarland ◽  
John Taylor ◽  
Terrence D. Hill ◽  
Katherine L. Friedman

2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
James Janford Li ◽  
Emily C. Hilton ◽  
Qiongshi Lu ◽  
Jinkuk Hong ◽  
Jan S. Greenberg ◽  
...  

Neuroticism is a stable and heritable personality trait that is strongly linked to depression. Yet, little is known about its association with late life depression, as well as how neuroticism eventuates into depression. This study used data from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS; N=4,877) to examine the direct and indirect effects of neuroticism on late life depression at three points in the life course – ages 53, 64, and 71 – via stressful life events (i.e., independent and dependent) and social supports measured across adulthood and into later life. Neuroticism was rigorously assayed using multiple methods, including self-report measures and a polygenic score informed by a meta-analytic genome-wide association study. Results indicated that the association between self-reported neuroticism and late life depression was partially mediated via the effects of dependent stressful life events experienced after the age of 53 and by age 64 (T2) social support. There were no specific indirect effects of self-reported neuroticism on depression through the effects of age 53 social support (T1), age 71 social support (T3), adult dependent stressful life events (experienced between age 19 and 52), and adult and late life independent stressful life events. These associations were replicated when we examined the direct and indirect effects of the polygenic score for neuroticism on late life depression, providing key evidence that our findings are robust. Results are consistent with previous findings that individuals with high neuroticism may be vulnerable to late life depression through psychosocial risk factors that are, in part, attributable to their own personality.


2012 ◽  
Vol 28 (8) ◽  
pp. 821-830 ◽  
Author(s):  
JoAnn T. Tschanz ◽  
Roxane Pfister ◽  
Joseph Wanzek ◽  
Chris Corcoran ◽  
Ken Smith ◽  
...  

2020 ◽  
Vol 32 (5) ◽  
pp. 1732-1742
Author(s):  
Mark Wade ◽  
Margaret A. Sheridan ◽  
Charles H. Zeanah ◽  
Nathan A. Fox ◽  
Charles A. Nelson ◽  
...  

AbstractChildren who spend their early lives in institutions experience profound psychosocial deprivation that is associated with altered stress response system development. Here, we used data from a longitudinal randomized controlled trial of foster care for institutionally reared children to examine whether caregiving quality and stressful life events (SLEs) in early adolescence (age 12) influence patterns of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and sympathetic nervous system (SNS) reactivity. Controlling for the effect of institutional care, higher caregiving quality at age 12 was associated with heightened cortisol and SNS reactivity. However, moderation analysis revealed that the latter effect was only observed among never-institutionalized children, whereas ever-institutionalized children demonstrated a persistently blunted SNS response regardless of recent caregiving quality. Among institutionally reared children, SLEs interacted with prior random assignment to foster care, such that those placed in foster care early in development had a SNS response that approximated never-institutionalized children when SLEs at age 12 were low. In contrast, SNS reactivity was persistently blunted among those with prolonged deprivation, regardless of recent SLEs. Early-life deprivation is associated with persistent blunting of stress response systems, but normalization may be achievable if SLEs are limited following placement into enriched family-based care.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document