ERP Evidence for Implicit Priming of Top–Down Control of Attention

2016 ◽  
Vol 28 (5) ◽  
pp. 763-772 ◽  
Author(s):  
Chris Blais ◽  
Emily Hubbard ◽  
George R. Mangun

Proportion congruency effects are the observation that the magnitude of the Stroop effect increases as the proportion of congruent trials in a block increases. Contemporary work shows that proportion effects can be specific to a particular context. For example, in a Simon task in which items appearing above fixation are mostly congruent and items appearing below fixation are mostly incongruent, the Simon effect is larger for the items appearing at the top. There is disagreement as to whether these context-specific effects result from simple associative learning or, instead, a type of conflict-mediated associative learning. Here, we address this question in an ERP study using a Simon task in which the proportion congruency effect was context-specific, manipulating the proportion of congruent trials based on location (upper vs. lower visual field). We found significant behavioral proportion congruency effects that varied with the specific contexts. In addition, we observed that the N2 response of the ERPs to the stimuli was larger in amplitude for the high congruent (high conflict) versus low congruent (low conflict) conditions/contexts. Because the N2 is known to be greater in amplitude also for trials where conflict is high and is believed to be an electrical signal related to conflict detection in the medial frontal cortex, this supports the idea that conflict-mediated associative learning is involved in the proportion congruency effect.

Author(s):  
Lilach Akiva-Kabiri ◽  
Avishai Henik

The Stroop task has been employed to study automaticity or failures of selective attention for many years. The effect is known to be asymmetrical, with words affecting color naming but not vice versa. In the current work two auditory-visual Stroop-like tasks were devised in order to study the automaticity of pitch processing in both absolute pitch (AP) possessors and musically trained controls without AP (nAP). In the tone naming task, participants were asked to name the auditory tone while ignoring a visual note name. In the note naming task, participants were asked to read a note name while ignoring the auditory tone. The nAP group showed a significant congruency effect only in the tone naming task, whereas AP possessors showed the reverse pattern, with a significant congruency effect only in the note reading task. Thus, AP possessors were unable to ignore the auditory tone when asked to read the note, but were unaffected by the verbal note name when asked to label the auditory tone. The results suggest that pitch identification in participants endowed with AP ability is automatic and impossible to suppress.


2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Fanny Grisetto ◽  
Yvonne N. Delevoye-Turrell ◽  
Clémence Roger

Aggressive behaviors in pathological and healthy populations have been largely related to poor cognitive control functioning. However, few studies investigated the influence of aggressive traits (i.e., aggressiveness) on cognitive control. In the current study, we investigated the effects of aggressiveness on cognitive control abilities and particularly, on performance monitoring. Thirty-two participants performed a Simon task while electroencephalography (EEG) and electromyography (EMG) were recorded. Participants were classified as high and low aggressive using the BPAQ questionnaire (Buss & Perry, 1992). EMG recordings were used to reveal three response types by uncovering small incorrect muscular activations in ~15% of correct trials (i.e., partial-errors) that have to be distinguished from full-error and pure-correct responses. For these three response types, EEG recordings were used to extract fronto-central negativities indicative of performance monitoring, the error and correct (-related) negativities (ERN/Ne and CRN/Nc). Behavioral results indicated that the high aggressiveness group had a larger congruency effect compared to the low aggressiveness group, but there were no differences in accuracy. EEG results revealed a global reduction in performance-related negativities amplitudes in all the response types in the high aggressiveness group compared to the low aggressiveness group. Interestingly, the distinction between the ERN/Ne and the CRN/Nc components was preserved both in high and low aggressiveness groups. In sum, high aggressive traits did not affect the capacity to self-evaluate erroneous from correct actions but are associated with a decrease in the importance given to one’s own performance. The implication of these findings are discussed in relation to pathological aggressiveness.


2002 ◽  
Vol 277 (30) ◽  
pp. 27367-27377 ◽  
Author(s):  
William P. Schiemann ◽  
Gerard C. Blobe ◽  
Dario E. Kalume ◽  
Akhilesh Pandey ◽  
Harvey F. Lodish

PLoS ONE ◽  
2019 ◽  
Vol 14 (7) ◽  
pp. e0214261
Author(s):  
Hannah S. Ferguson ◽  
Anya Owen ◽  
Amanda C. Hahn ◽  
Jaimie Torrance ◽  
Lisa M. DeBruine ◽  
...  

Blood ◽  
2015 ◽  
Vol 126 (23) ◽  
pp. 843-843
Author(s):  
Jeffrey A. Magee ◽  
Shaina Porter ◽  
Andrew Cluster

Abstract Much progress has been made toward identifying the mutations that cause human acute myeloid leukemia (AML), and these studies have shown that pediatric and adult AML are often caused by different mutations. Genetic differences between pediatric and adult AML may underlie differences in outcomes and necessitate different treatment strategies, yet we have few insights into why these differences occur. One possibility is that the mechanisms that regulate normal hematopoiesis change with age, and mutations therefore have age-specific phenotypes in pre-leukemic progenitors. To fully understand how AML evolves in children and adults, and how targeting individual pathways might impact cell physiology at different ages, it is important to understand how somatic mutations interface with the normal, temporally dynamic programs that regulate hematopoiesis. The FLT3-Internal Tandem Duplication (FLT3-ITD) mutation is common in adult AML but rare in early childhood AML (30-40% of adult AML, 5-10% of AML in children <10 years old, <1% of infant AML). FLT3-ITD mutations occur late in the clonal evolution of AML cells, and they are thought to drive cell proliferation and survival. In mice, FLT3-ITD has been shown to deplete adult hematopoietic stem cells (HSCs) by promoting myeloid differentiation. This may explain why the mutation occurs late in clonal evolution - HSCs must first acquire mutations that enhance self-renewal - but it also raises the question of why infant HSCs, which have an inherently higher self-renewal capacity, do not give rise to FLT3-ITD positive AML more often than is observed. We used FLT3-ITD knock-in mice to test whether FLT3-ITD has developmental context specific effects on hematopoiesis. In adult mice, FLT3-ITD depleted the HSC pool and expanded multipotent progenitor (MPP) and myeloid progenitor populations consistent with prior studies. In fetal mice, FLT3-ITD had no effect on HSC or MPP numbers, HSC function (as determined by limit dilution transplants) or myelopoiesis. FLT3-ITD did not affect hematopoiesis until shortly after birth. These temporal differences were evident even in the presence of cooperating Runx1 mutations. To understand why fetal and adult progenitors responded differently to FLT3-ITD, we characterized signal transduction and gene expression in fetal, neonatal and adult progenitors. We found that STAT5 was activated by FLT3-ITD at all stages of development, but MAPK was activated only in post-natal progenitors concordant with the onset of HSC and myeloid phenotypes. To our surprise, conditional Stat5a/b deletion exacerbated the HSC depletion and myeloid expansion phenotypes of adult FLT3-ITD mice rather than rescuing them. This suggests that STAT5 helps to maintain adult, FLT3-ITD mutant progenitors in an undifferentiated state even as other effectors promote myeloid differentiation. We next used microarrays to test whether FLT3-ITD has age-specific effects on gene expression in HSCs and MPPs, and to identify normal temporal changes in gene expression that may modulate the FLT3-ITD phenotypes. These studies made several key points: 1) In wild type HSCs, most fetal-specific genes were inactivated and most adult-specific genes were activated between birth and P14. This transition was earlier than prior studies have suggested, and it correlated with the age at which FLT3-ITD induced HSC depletion and myeloid expansion. 2) FLT3-ITD did not alter gene expression until after birth, coincident with onset of the HSC depletion and myeloid expansion phenotypes. 3) FLT3-ITD target genes were more differentially expressed in MPPs than in HSCs, consistent with recent data suggesting that MPPs are a cell of origin for FLT3-ITD driven AML. 4) Most, but not all, FLT3-ITD target genes were STAT5 dependent. Our analyses have identified novel, adult-specific candidate effectors of FLT3-ITD. Moreover, our findings raise the question of whether fetal genetic programs can suppress FLT3-ITD driven leukemogenesis, and we have begun to address this question with gain of function models. AML cells may exhibit "context addiction" (i.e.a sustained requirement for normal adult gene products and a toxic response to fetal gene products), that could be exploited therapeutically. Disclosures No relevant conflicts of interest to declare.


2020 ◽  
Vol 117 (52) ◽  
pp. 33700-33710
Author(s):  
Katherine Domb ◽  
Aviva Katz ◽  
Keith D. Harris ◽  
Rafael Yaari ◽  
Efrat Kaisler ◽  
...  

Cytosine (DNA) methylation in plants regulates the expression of genes and transposons. While methylation in plant genomes occurs at CG, CHG, and CHH sequence contexts, the comparative roles of the individual methylation contexts remain elusive. Here, we present Physcomitrella patens as the second plant system, besides Arabidopsis thaliana, with viable mutants with an essentially complete loss of methylation in the CG and non-CG contexts. In contrast to A. thaliana, P. patens has more robust CHH methylation, similar CG and CHG methylation levels, and minimal cross-talk between CG and non-CG methylation, making it possible to study context-specific effects independently. Our data found CHH methylation to act in redundancy with symmetric methylation in silencing transposons and to regulate the expression of CG/CHG-depleted transposons. Specific elimination of CG methylation did not dysregulate transposons or genes. In contrast, exclusive removal of non-CG methylation massively up-regulated transposons and genes. In addition, comparing two exclusively but equally CG- or CHG-methylated genomes, we show that CHG methylation acts as a greater transcriptional regulator than CG methylation. These results disentangle the transcriptional roles of CG and non-CG, as well as symmetric and asymmetric methylation in a plant genome, and point to the crucial role of non-CG methylation in genome regulation.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Guanglin Zhang ◽  
Qingying Meng ◽  
Montgomery Blencowe ◽  
Agrawal Rahul ◽  
Fernando Gomez-Pinilla ◽  
...  

AbstractScopeWe explored the influence of DHA on cardiometabolic and cognitive phenotypes, and multiomic alterations in the brain under two metabolic conditions to understand context-specific nutritional effects.Methods and ResultsRats were randomly assigned to a DHA-rich or a control chow diet while drinking water or high fructose solution, followed by profiling of metabolic and cognitive phenotypes and the transcriptome and DNA methylome of the hypothalamus and hippocampus. DHA reduced serum triglyceride and improved insulin resistance and memory exclusively in the fructose-consuming rats. In hippocampus, DHA affected genes related to synapse functions in the chow group but immune functions in the fructose group; in hypothalamus, DHA altered immune pathways in the chow group but metabolic pathways in the fructose group. Network modeling revealed context-specific regulators of DHA effects, including Klf4 and Dusp1 for chow condition and Lum, Fn1, and Col1a1 for fructose condition in hippocampus, as well as Cyr61, JunB, Ier2, and Pitx2 under chow condition and Hcar1, Cdh1, and Osr1 under fructose condition in hypothalamus.ConclusionDHA exhibits differential influence on epigenetic loci, genes, pathways, and metabolic and cognitive phenotypes under different dietary contexts, supporting population stratification in DHA studies to achieve precision nutrition.


Author(s):  
Lilach Akiva-Kabiri ◽  
Avishai Henik

In their paper “The Musical Stroop Effect: Opening a New Avenue to Research on Automatisms,” Grégoire, Perruchet, and Poulin-Charronnat (2013) use a musical Stroop-like task to demonstrate the automaticity of musical note naming in musicians. In addition, the authors suggest that music training can serve as a tool in order to study the acquisition of automaticity. In the following commentary, we aim to address three main issues concerning the paper by Grégoire et al. (2013) . First, we will suggest some additional interpretations of the results; specifically, we will relate to the association between music and space. Second, we will discuss a methodological issue dealing with interference, facilitation, and the role of the neutral condition. We suggest that the study by Grégoire et al. (2013) lacks a proper neutral condition and thus it is impossible to assert that the congruency effect is interference based. Third, we will discuss the authors’ suggestion of using the musical Stroop effect as a tool for studying automatism. We consider the practical relevance of music training as a tool for studying the acquisition of automaticity by pointing out that music training is highly heterogeneous.


2018 ◽  
Vol 72 (5) ◽  
pp. 1119-1130 ◽  
Author(s):  
James R Schmidt ◽  
Céline Lemercier

Conflict between task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimulus information leads to impairment in response speed and accuracy. For instance, in the colour-word Stroop paradigm, participants respond slower and less accurately to the print colour of incongruent colour words (e.g., “red” printed in green) than to congruent colour words (e.g., “green” in green). Importantly, this congruency effect is diminished when the trials in an experiment are mostly incongruent, relative to mostly congruent, termed a proportion congruent effect. When distracting stimuli are mostly congruent in one context (e.g., location or font) but mostly incongruent in another context (e.g., another location or font), the congruency effect is still diminished in the mostly incongruent context, termed a context-specific proportion congruent (CSPC) effect. Both the standard proportion congruent and CSPC effects are typically interpreted in terms of conflict-driven attentional control, frequently termed conflict adaptation or conflict monitoring. However, in two experiments, we investigated contingency learning confounds in context-specific proportion congruent effects. In particular, two variants of a dissociation procedure are presented with the font variant of the CSPC procedure. In both, robust contingency learning effects were observed. No evidence for context-specific control was observed. In fact, results trended in the wrong direction. In all, the results suggest that CSPC effects may not be a useful way of studying attentional control.


Cell Reports ◽  
2015 ◽  
Vol 13 (11) ◽  
pp. 2480-2490 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ana Tufegdzic Vidakovic ◽  
Oscar M. Rueda ◽  
Stephin J. Vervoort ◽  
Ankita Sati Batra ◽  
Mae Akilina Goldgraben ◽  
...  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document