fixed screen
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

7
(FIVE YEARS 3)

H-INDEX

1
(FIVE YEARS 0)

2021 ◽  
Vol 9 (6) ◽  
pp. 523-532
Author(s):  
Vladimir Kondratenko ◽  
Gaik Sagatelyan ◽  
Andrey Shishlov ◽  
Mikhail Bilinkin

The possibilities of technological ensuring of the uniformity of thickness distribution of a thin-film metal coating produced by magnetron sputtering on the inner surface of a thinwalled silica resonator made in the shape of a hemisphere are considered. The possibility of minimizing the thickness of the coating by optimizing the diameter of the annular magnetron emission zone in combination with the distance from the resonator to the target made of sprayed material is shown. A further increase in the evenness of thickness of the coating is possible on the basis of the use of a fixed screen with a hole, the shape and location of which are calculated analytically, and the final configuration of the contour is specified empirically


2021 ◽  
Vol 9 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lihua Huang ◽  
Katrina L. Schmid ◽  
Xiao-Na Yin ◽  
Jingyu Zhang ◽  
Jianbo Wu ◽  
...  

Evidence regarding screen use and outdoor activity during very early childhood (i. e., from aged 1 to 3 years) and their potential combined links to the later preschool myopia is limited. This information is needed to release effective public health messages and propose intervention strategies against preschool myopia. We collected information regarding very early childhood screen use, outdoor activity and the kindergartens vision screenings of 26,611 preschoolers from Longhua Child Cohort Study by questionnaires. Logistic regression models were used to examine the associations between reported outdoor activity, screen use from 1 to 3 years of age, and preschool myopia. Throughout very early childhood, from 1 to 3 years, the proportion of children exposed to screens increased (from 35.8 to 68.4%, p < 0.001), whereas the proportion of children who went outdoors ≥7 times/week (67.4–62.1%, p < 0.001) and who went outdoors for ≥60 min/time (53.3–38.0%, p < 0.001) declined. Exposure to fixed screen devices [adjusted odds ratio (AOR) = 2.66, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 2.09–3.44], mobile screen devices (AOR = 2.76, 95% CI = 2.15–3.58), and limited outdoor activity (AOR = 1.87, 95% CI = 1.42–2.51) during early childhood were associated with preschool myopia. Among children whose parents were myopic, the interactions between outdoor activity and fixed or mobile screen use on later preschool myopia were significant; the ORs and 95% CI were 3.34 (1.19–9.98) and 3.04 (1.06–9.21), respectively. Our findings suggest the possibility that the impact of screen exposure during early childhood on preschool myopia could be diminished by outdoor activity for children whose parents have myopia.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
William W. Abbott ◽  
J. Alex Harston ◽  
A. Aldo Faisal

AbstractGaze behaviour and motor actions are fundamentally interlinked in both a spatial and temporal manner. However, the vast majority of gaze behaviour research has focused to date on reductionist head-fixed screen viewing experiments and ignored the motor aspect of visuomotor behaviour, thereby neglecting a critical component of the perception-action loop. We address this with an experimental design to capture, rather than constrain, the full range of simultaneous gaze and motor behaviour in a range of natural daily life tasks. Through building autoregressive models and applying these to our novel datasets we find that beyond simple static regions of interest, we can predict visual attention shifts from freely-moving first person body kinematics, through explaining gaze dynamics in the context of body dynamics, on the timescale of freely moving interactive behaviour in individuals, expanding our understanding of natural visuomotor behaviour.


Author(s):  
Haroldo F. de Araujo ◽  
Paulo A. M. Leal ◽  
Thais Q. Zorzeto ◽  
Eduardo F. Nunes ◽  
Pâmela S. Betin

ABSTRACT The objective of this research was to evaluate the meteorological elements of the environments and the production components of mini tomato crop in organic system in different forms of cultivation, biofertilizer doses and technological levels of protected environments. The research was conducted in completely randomized design in a 3 x 2 x 5 factorial scheme, corresponding to three greenhouses (A - climatized, B - mobile screen and C - fixed screen), two forms of cultivation (pots and beds) and five doses of a commercial biofertilizer (0, 50, 100, 150 and 200% of dose indicated), with five replicates. The treatments affected the analyzed variables, except for biofertilizer doses and interaction of treatments. Air temperature was unchanged among the environments and the mean and minimum relative humidity were within the control range in the climatized greenhouse. The greenhouse with mobile screen showed the best production results for the cultivation in beds and the biofertilizer doses were indifferent for all treatments.


2010 ◽  
Vol 51 (1) ◽  
pp. 156-164 ◽  
Author(s):  
Tatsuya Oki ◽  
Taeko Hazumi ◽  
Yoko Umemiya ◽  
Mikio Kobayashi

Author(s):  
Richard S. Laymon

A comparison was made of two methods of displaying map information to image interpreters in a tactical image interpretation context. While viewing each display, interpreters located the boundary of an aerial photograph and then estimated the map coordinates of a designated object on the same photograph. Time taken to achieve correct solutions was compared when map information was displayed on simulated projected maps (map sections were posted against a fixed screen) and on standard map sheets. The latter mode was structured so that adjoining maps were always combined when the photographic area covered two adjoining maps. The map-photograph viewing relationship was varied in two ways: one in which the position of the photograph was fixed and one in which the photograph could be freely oriented to the map display. The following conclusions were derived: (1) Simulated projected maps increase the time an interpreter takes to find a photograph's area on a map and to determine map coordinates of an object on the photograph; the longer time is attributable to the need to view successively two projected maps when the photographic area lies near the boundary of a projected map. (2) Freedom to orient a photograph relative to a map display does not shorten the time taken to find the photographic area on a map or to determine map coordinates of a designated object on the photograph.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document