Visual perception: An event over time.

1957 ◽  
Vol 64 (5) ◽  
pp. 306-313 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gudmund Smith
Keyword(s):  
2019 ◽  
Vol 4 (2) ◽  
pp. 60
Author(s):  
Nadya Nilafianty Prasetya ◽  
Maria Immaculata Ririk Winandari

ABSTRACT The development of the tourism industry in Indonesia needs to be supported by appropriate facilities and infrastructure. Hotel as one of the supporting tourism in Indonesia has to be properly expanded. According to the data from Central Bureau of Statistics (BPS), the occupancy rate of star-rated hotels continues to increase over time. One of the factors affecting the customer's decision in choosing a hotel is its interior design. Interior elements consist of floor, wall, ceiling, and furniture. The wall element is one of the interior elements that are attractive to visitors. To find out the perception of hotel visitors, the author surveyed five three-star hotels in Jakarta. The five hotels are Maxone hotel in Matraman, Yellow Hotel in Harmoni, Lynt Hotel in Gambir, Park 5 Hotel and Swissbellinn both are located in Simatupang. The method used in this study is a mixed-method with a visual perception approach in the form of direct interviews and distributed questionnaires to 40 respondents. The results of the research show that several wall criteria of the hotel that are suitable for visitors among others are: bright wall colors and walls with soothing color schemes. Keywords: Guest’s preferences, hotel rooms, visual perception, wall design


2010 ◽  
Vol 104 (5) ◽  
pp. 2886-2899 ◽  
Author(s):  
Thaddeus B. Czuba ◽  
Bas Rokers ◽  
Alexander C. Huk ◽  
Lawrence K. Cormack

Two binocular cues are thought to underlie the visual perception of three-dimensional (3D) motion: a disparity-based cue, which relies on changes in disparity over time, and a velocity-based cue, which relies on interocular velocity differences. The respective building blocks of these cues, instantaneous disparity and retinal motion, exhibit very distinct spatial and temporal signatures. Although these two cues are synchronous in naturally moving objects, disparity-based and velocity-based mechanisms can be dissociated experimentally. We therefore investigated how the relative contributions of these two cues change across a range of viewing conditions. We measured direction-discrimination sensitivity for motion though depth across a wide range of eccentricities and speeds for disparity-based stimuli, velocity-based stimuli, and “full cue” stimuli containing both changing disparities and interocular velocity differences. Surprisingly, the pattern of sensitivity for velocity-based stimuli was nearly identical to that for full cue stimuli across the entire extent of the measured spatiotemporal surface and both were clearly distinct from those for the disparity-based stimuli. These results suggest that for direction discrimination outside the fovea, 3D motion perception primarily relies on the velocity-based cue with little, if any, contribution from the disparity-based cue.


Vision ◽  
2018 ◽  
Vol 2 (3) ◽  
pp. 27 ◽  
Author(s):  
Louise O’Hare

Migraine is associated with differences in visual perception, specifically, deficits in the perception of motion. Migraine groups commonly show poorer performance (higher thresholds) on global motion tasks compared to control groups. Successful performance on a global motion task depends on several factors, including integrating signals over time. A “motion streak” task was used to investigate specifically integration over time in migraine and control groups. The motion streak effect depends on the integration of a moving point over time to create the illusion of a line, or “streak”. There was evidence of a slower optimum speed for eliciting the motion streak effect in migraine compared to control groups, suggesting temporal integration is different in migraine. In addition, performance on the motion streak task showed a relationship with headache frequency.


2014 ◽  
Vol 14 (10) ◽  
pp. 457-457
Author(s):  
J. J. R. van Assen ◽  
V. C. Paulun ◽  
R. W. Fleming

Perception ◽  
1996 ◽  
Vol 25 (1_suppl) ◽  
pp. 171-171 ◽  
Author(s):  
J Wagemans ◽  
S Tibau

Lappin and Fuqua (1983 Science221 480 – 482) reported accurate measurement of three-dimensional (3-D) distances and suggested that performance was based on structural invariance of patterns undergoing perspective transformations. We wanted to replicate their basic findings and test specific hypotheses about the use of Euclidean cues versus affine or projective invariants. The displays consisted of orthographic and perspective projections of three collinear dots rotating rigidly around a fixed centre in a plane slanted 45° in depth. Observers were asked to decide whether the middle of the three dots was exactly centred in 3-D space between the other two dots. The visible rotation segments were 120°, 160°, or 200° and the displacements were 2%, 4%, or 6%. Although our untrained observers performed more poorly overall than Lappin's well-practiced observers, two main results were replicated. First, there was no effect of segment size, which suggests that Euclidean cues were not used. Second, there was no difference between orthographic and perspective displays, which suggests that the invariants used must be projective, not affine. Additional experiments will be performed to investigate the relative importance of invariants such as cross ratios, which are based on the positions of the dots within each time frame, versus invariants of the conic sections, which are based on the elliptic traces of the dots formed over time.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
James A Belarde ◽  
Claire W Chen ◽  
Elizabeth Rafikian ◽  
Mu Yang ◽  
Carol M Troy

For the last twenty years, the Bussey-Saksida touchscreen-based operant conditioning platform has evolved in close parallel alongside the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) to produce batteries of tests for studying complex cognitive functions in rodents that are increasingly analogous to human diagnostic tests and greatly narrow the translational gap in cognition research. Naturally, with this increasing usefulness comes increasing use, and with it, a need for a greater understanding of and controlling for confounding factors that may limit the system's ability to optimally detect cognitive deficits. In the present study, we show a strong image preference bias in a standard pairwise discrimination task with a widely used spider-plane image pairing. This bias greatly influenced the performance of our experimental mice, significantly affecting the length of time it took mice to complete the task, their progress over time, and several accessory measures usefully recorded by the Bussey-Saksida touchscreen system. We further show that this bias can be corrected by using more highly similar image pairings without sacrificing the animal's ability to learn to distinguish the stimuli. This eliminated all significant stimuli specific differences seen with the spider-plane pairing. We then analyze the pixel composition of the various stimuli to suggest that the bias is due to a difference in image brightness. These findings highlight the importance of carefully modulating paired touchscreen stimuli to ensure equivalence prior to learning and the need for more studies of visual perception in mice, particularly as it relates to their performance in cognitive assays.


Author(s):  
Colin Webster

The chapter surveys the scope of ancient optics, which varied over time and between authors. No two authors in antiquity agree on precisely which elements should be included in a useful and credible account of visual perception. The chapter adopts a holistic approach that pays attention to how each author functionally defines sight. By the time of Plato, a relatively consistent set of phenomena defined sight, including color, image transfer, reflection, and night vision. Aristotle incorporated sight within a broader metaphysical account of perception and the soul, while the Epicureans fixated on the epistemological consequences of optical illusions. In the Hellenistic period, geometrical optics (and its sibling discipline catoptrics, the study of mirrors) rose to greater prominence, utilizing diagrams to explain and model vision. Ptolemy composed a treatise that systematized and synthesized both the geometrical and philosophical approaches to sight.


2018 ◽  
Vol 41 ◽  
Author(s):  
David Hirshleifer ◽  
Siew Hong Teoh

AbstractEvolved dispositions influence, but do not determine, how people think about economic problems. The evolutionary cognitive approach offers important insights but underweights the social transmission of ideas as a level of explanation. The need for asocialexplanation for the evolution of economic attitudes is evidenced, for example, by immense variations in folk-economic beliefs over time and across individuals.


1988 ◽  
Vol 19 (3) ◽  
pp. 251-258 ◽  
Author(s):  
Virginia I. Wolfe ◽  
Suzanne D. Blocker ◽  
Norma J. Prater

Articulatory generalization of velar cognates /k/, /g/ in two phonologically disordered children was studied over time as a function of sequential word-morpheme position training. Although patterns of contextual acquisition differed, correct responses to the word-medial, inflected context (e.g., "picking," "hugging") occurred earlier and exceeded those to the word-medial, noninflected context (e.g., "bacon," "wagon"). This finding indicates that the common view of the word-medial position as a unitary concept is an oversimplification. Possible explanations for superior generalization to the word-medial, inflected position are discussed in terms of coarticulation, perceptual salience, and the representational integrity of the word.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document