scholarly journals Orientation to polarized light in tethered flying honeybees

2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Norihiro Kobayashi ◽  
Ryuichi Okada ◽  
Midori Sakura

ABSTRACTBehavioral responses of honeybees to a zenithal polarized light stimulus were observed using a tethered animal in a flight simulator. Flight direction of the bee was recorded by monitoring the horizontal movement of its abdomen, which was strongly anti-correlated with its torque. When the e-vector orientation of the polarized light was rotated clockwise or counterclockwise, the bee responded with periodic right-and-left abdominal movements; however, the bee did not show any clear periodic movement under the static e-vector or depolarized stimulus. The steering frequency of the bee was well coordinated with the e-vector rotation frequency of the stimulus, indicating that the flying bee oriented itself to a certain e-vector orientation, i.e., exhibited polarotaxis. The percentage of bees exhibiting clear polarotaxis was much smaller under the fast stimulus (3.6 ° s-1) compared with that of the slow stimulus (0.9 or 1.8 ° s-1). The bee did not demonstrate any polarotactic behavior after the dorsal rim region of its eyes, which mediates insect polarization vision in general, was bilaterally covered with black paint. The bees demonstrated a high preference for e-vector orientations between 120 to 180°. Each bee exhibited similar e-vector preferences under clockwise and counterclockwise stimuli, indicating that each bee has its own e-vector preference, which probably depends on the bee’s previous foraging experience. Our results strongly suggest that the flying honeybees utilize the e-vector information from the skylight to deduce their heading orientation for navigation.Summary statementTethered flying bees exhibited polarotaxis under a zenithal rotating e-vector stimulus, in which their right-and-left abdominal movements were coincident with the rotation of the stimulus.

2020 ◽  
Vol 223 (23) ◽  
pp. jeb228254
Author(s):  
Norihiro Kobayashi ◽  
Ryuichi Okada ◽  
Midori Sakura

ABSTRACTMany insects exploit the partial plane polarization of skylight for visual compass orientation and/or navigation. In the present study, using a tethering system, we investigated how flying bees respond to polarized light stimuli. The behavioral responses of honeybees (Apis mellifera) to a zenithal polarized light stimulus were observed using a tethered animal in a flight simulator. Flight direction of the bee was recorded by monitoring the horizontal movement of its abdomen, which was strongly anti-correlated with its torque. When the e-vector orientation of the polarized light was rotated clockwise or counterclockwise, the bee responded with periodic right-and-left abdominal movements; however, the bee did not show any clear periodic movement under the static e-vector or depolarized stimulus. The steering frequency of the bee was well coordinated with the e-vector rotation frequency of the stimulus, indicating that the flying bee oriented itself to a certain e-vector orientation, i.e. exhibited polarotaxis. The percentage of bees exhibiting clear polarotaxis was much smaller under the fast stimulus (3.6 deg s−1) compared with that under a slow stimulus (0.9 or 1.8 deg s−1). Bees did not demonstrate any polarotactic behavior after the dorsal rim area of the eyes, which mediates insect polarization vision in general, was bilaterally covered with black paint. Preferred e-vector orientations under the clockwise stimulus varied among individuals and distributed throughout −90 to 90 deg. Some bees showed similar preferred e-vector orientations for clockwise and counterclockwise stimuli whereas others did not. Our results strongly suggest that flying honeybees utilize the e-vector information from the skylight to deduce their heading orientation for navigation.


2011 ◽  
Vol 279 (1728) ◽  
pp. 535-542 ◽  
Author(s):  
Midori Sakura ◽  
Ryuichi Okada ◽  
Hitoshi Aonuma

Many insects use the polarization pattern of the sky for obtaining compass information during orientation or navigation. E-vector information is collected by a specialized area in the dorsal-most part of the compound eye, the dorsal rim area (DRA). We tested honeybees' capability of learning certain e-vector orientations by using a classical conditioning paradigm with the proboscis extension reflex. When one e-vector orientation (CS+) was associated with sugar water, while another orientation (CS−) was not rewarded, the honeybees could discriminate CS+ from CS−. Bees whose DRA was inactivated by painting did not learn CS+. When ultraviolet (UV) polarized light (350 nm) was used for CS, the bees discriminated CS+ from CS−, but no discrimination was observed in blue (442 nm) or green light (546 nm). Our data indicate that honeybees can learn and discriminate between different e-vector orientations, sensed by the UV receptors of the DRA, suggesting that bees can determine their flight direction from polarized UV skylight during foraging. Fixing the bees' heads during the experiments did not prevent learning, indicating that they use an ‘instantaneous’ algorithm of e-vector detection; that is, the bees do not need to actively scan the sky with their DRAs (‘sequential’ method) to determine e-vector orientation.


1996 ◽  
Vol 199 (7) ◽  
pp. 1467-1475 ◽  
Author(s):  
T Labhart

In crickets, polarized-light information from the blue sky is processed by polarization-opponent interneurones (POL-neurones). These neurones receive input from the polarization-sensitive blue receptors found in the specialized dorsal rim area of the compound eye. Even under optimal conditions, the degree of polarization d does not exceed 0.75 in the blue region of the spectrum and it is normally much lower. The aim of this study is to assess how POL-neurones perform at low, physiologically relevant degrees of polarization. The spiking activity of POL-neurones is a sinusoidal function of e-vector orientation with a 180 ° period. The modulation amplitude of this function decreases strongly as the degree of polarization decreases. However, our data indicate that POL-neurones can signal e-vector information at d-values as low as 0.05, which would allow the polarization-sensitive system of crickets to exploit polarized light from the sky for orientation even under unfavourable meteorological conditions.


1998 ◽  
Vol 80 (5) ◽  
pp. 2571-2583 ◽  
Author(s):  
Raymon M. Glantz ◽  
Andy McIsaac

Glantz, Raymon M. and Andy McIsaac. Two-channel polarization analyzer in the sustaining fiber-dimming fiber ensemble of crayfish visual system. J. Neurophysiol. 80: 2571–2583, 1998. Polarization sensitivity (PS) was examined in two classes of neurons, sustaining fibers and dimming fibers, in the medulla externa (second optic neuropile) of the crayfish, Pacifasticus leniusculus. Visual responses were recorded intracellularly and extracellularly. The influence of e-vector orientation (θ) was probed in steady-state responses, with brief flashes and with a rotating polarizer. The results indicate that the entire sustaining fiber population appears to be maximally sensitive to vertically polarized light. Although the evidence is less complete for dimming fibers, they appear to be maximally inhibited by vertically polarized light and excited by horizontally polarized light. Thus the sustaining fibers and dimming fibers form a two-channel polarization analyzer that captures the main features of the polarization system established in photoreceptors and lamina monopolar cells. The available evidence suggests that this two-channel system has the same characteristics across most or all of the retinula. Lateral inhibition in sustaining fibers is differentially sensitive to θ. Inhibition is substantial at θ = 90° (horizontal) and essentially absent at θ = 0°. The details of the sustaining fiber polarization response closely follow features established in more peripheral neurons, including the magnitude of PS, enhanced responsiveness to a changing e-vector, and modest directionality to a changing e-vector in∼40% of the cells.


1982 ◽  
Vol 96 (1) ◽  
pp. 69-91 ◽  
Author(s):  
MICHAEL L. BRINES ◽  
JAMES L. GOULD

1. Although many invertebrate animals orient by means of ultraviolet sky-light polarization patterns, existing measurements of these patterns are inadequate for full analysis of the biologically relevant information available from the sky. To fill this gap we have used a precision scanning polarimeter to measure simultaneously the intensity, degree, and direction of vibration (E-vector orientation) of polarized light at 5° intervals over the sky. The resulting sky maps were constructed for u.v. (350 nm) and visible wavelengths (500 and 650 nm) under a variety of atmospheric conditions. 2. Our measurements confirmed that the patterns of radiance and degree of polarization of skylight are highly variable and hence unreliable as orientation cues; but patterns of E-vector orientation are relatively stable and predictable over most of the sky under all but very hazy or overcast conditions. 3. The observed E-vector patterns correspond more closely to predictions based on first order (Rayleigh) scattering at 650 and 500 nm than at 350 nm. This is true both in terms of absolute accuracy and the proportion of the sky with relatively ‘correct’ information. Yet most insects respond to polarization patterns only at u.v. wavelengths. This apparent paradox can perhaps be resolved by assuming that there is no great selective advantage for any particular wavelength when large areas of blue sky are visible, but that under special and difficult conditions ultraviolet has advantages over longer wavelengths. Measurements under partially cloud-covered sky, for instance, or under extensive vegetation, show that both spuriously polarized and unpolarized light resulting from reflexions present more troublesome interference at longer wavelengths than in the u.v. 4. The accuracy of orientation achieved by dancing honey bees appears to be greater than can readily be accounted for by assuming that they use a strictly geometrical or analytical processing system for their orientation to polarized light.


2019 ◽  
Vol 627 ◽  
pp. L10 ◽  
Author(s):  
R. Brunngräber ◽  
S. Wolf

Investigation of the polarized light of protoplanetary disks is key for constraining dust properties, disk morphology, and embedded magnetic fields. However, different polarization mechanisms and the diversity of dust grain shapes and compositions lead to ambiguities in the polarization pattern. The so-called “self-scattering” of thermal, re-emitted radiation in the infrared and millimetre and submillimetre wavelengths is discussed as a major polarization mechanism. If the net flux of the radiation field is in the radial direction, it is commonly assumed that the polarization pattern produced by scattering in a protoplanetary disk shows concentric rings for disks seen in face-on orientation. We show that a change of 90° of the polarization vector orientation may occur and mimic the typical pattern of dichroic emission of dust grains aligned by a toroidal magnetic field in disks seen close to face-on. Furthermore, this effect of polarization reversal is a fast-changing function of wavelength and grain size, and is thus a powerful tool to constrain grain composition and size distribution present in protoplanetary disks. In addition, the effect may also provide unique constraints for the disk inclination, especially if the disk is seen close to face-on.


2001 ◽  
Vol 79 (3) ◽  
pp. 407-415 ◽  
Author(s):  
Shelee L Degner ◽  
Craig W Hawryshyn

Orientation responses of juvenile rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) to two linearly polarized light patches were examined under controlled laboratory conditions. Fish were trained to swim the length of the training tank under a polarized light field created by two linearly polarized stimuli that were oriented either parallel or perpendicular to the length of the tank. Trained fish were released in a circular tank and their angular responses were recorded. For each testing paradigm, the E-vector (electric vector) orientation of one of the two linearly polarized light patches was varied by 15° between 0° and 90°. Each fish was therefore tested in seven different paradigms in which the two E-vector orientations differed by 0°, 15°, 30°, 45°, 60°, 75°, and 90°. Rainbow trout oriented in a bimodal distribution when the two E-vector orientations differed by 0°, 15°, 30°, 45°, and 90°. These results suggest that rainbow trout perceived the two stimuli as being the same when the two E-vector orientations differed by 45° or less. Conversely, rainbow trout did not significantly orient when the two E-vector orientations differed by 60° and 75°. Rainbow trout may be able to discriminate two E-vector orientations that differ between 60° and 75°, and therefore they do not significantly orient, since they perceive two distinct E-vectors to orient to instead of one. When rainbow trout were exposed to a depolarized light field, they did not exhibit significant orientation subsequent to the E-vector cue.


1994 ◽  
Vol 191 (1) ◽  
pp. 107-123 ◽  
Author(s):  
M Coemans ◽  
J Hzn ◽  
J Nuboer

Orientation with reference to the time-compensated sun-azimuth compass has been established for the homing pigeon Columba livia. Previous qualitative studies claim that pigeons are sensitive to the orientation of a polarizer and it has been suggested that these animals are able to use sky-light polarization as an indirect reference to the sun's position when the latter is shielded from view. We report experiments which were undertaken to quantify the sensitivity of the homing pigeon to the orientation of linearly polarized light. The results of our initial experiments suggested that the animals responded to secondary cues. Further experiments were carried out to avoid such artefacts. Under circumstances where secondary cues were rigorously avoided, we were, however, not able to demonstrate any directional response that was caused by the E-vector orientation of the illumination. These results throw doubt on the suggested polarization-sensitivity of birds in general.


Author(s):  
L. D. Ackerman ◽  
S. H. Y. Wei

Mature human dental enamel has presented investigators with several difficulties in ultramicrotomy of specimens for electron microscopy due to its high degree of mineralization. This study explores the possibility of combining ion-milling and high voltage electron microscopy as a means of circumventing the problems of ultramicrotomy.A longitudinal section of an extracted human third molar was ground to a thickness of about 30 um and polarized light micrographs were taken. The specimen was attached to a single hole grid and thinned by argon-ion bombardment at 15° incidence while rotating at 15 rpm. The beam current in each of two guns was 50 μA with an accelerating voltage of 4 kV. A 20 nm carbon coating was evaporated onto the specimen to prevent an electron charge from building up during electron microscopy.


Author(s):  
Vicki L. Baliga ◽  
Mary Ellen Counts

Calcium is an important element in the growth and development of plants and one form of calcium is calcium oxalate. Calcium oxalate has been found in leaf seed, stem material plant tissue culture, fungi and lichen using one or more of the following methods—polarized light microscopy (PLM), scanning electron microscopy (SEM), transmission electron microscopy (TEM) and x-ray diffraction.Two methods are presented here for qualitatively estimating calcium oxalate in dried or fixed tobacco (Nicotiana) leaf from different stalk positions using PLM. SEM, coupled with energy dispersive x-ray spectrometry (EDS), and powder x-ray diffraction were used to verify that the crystals observed in the dried leaf with PLM were calcium oxalate.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document