scholarly journals Event and Entity Coreference Across Five Languages: Effects of Context and Referring Expression

2021 ◽  
Vol 12 (2) ◽  
pp. 192-226
Author(s):  
Luca Bevacqua ◽  
Sharid Loáiciga ◽  
Hannah Rohde ◽  
Christian Hardmeier

Current work on coreference focuses primarily on entities, often leaving unanalysed the use of anaphors to corefer with antecedents such as events and textual segments. Moreover, the anaphoric forms that speakers use for entity and non-entity coreference are not mutually exclusive. This ambiguity has been the subject of recent work in English, with evidence of a split between comprehenders' preferential interpretation of personal versus demonstrative pronouns. In addition, comprehenders are shown to be sensitive to antecedent complexity and aspectual status, two verb-driven cues that signal how an event is being portrayed. Here we extend this work via a comparison across five languages (English, French, German, Italian, and Spanish). With a story-continuation experiment, we test how different referring expressions corefer with entity and event antecedents and whether verbal features such as argument structure and aspect influence this choice. Our results show widely consistent, not categorical biases across languages: entity coreference is favoured for personal pronouns and event coreference for demonstratives. Antecedent complexity increases the rate at which anaphors are taken to corefer with an event antecedent, but portraying an event as completed does not reach statistical significance (though showing quite uniform patterns). Lastly, we report a comparison of the same referring expressions to refer to entity and event antecedents in a trilingual parallel corpus annotated with coreference.Together, the results provide a first crosslingual picture of coreference preferences beyond the restricted entity-only patterns targeted by most existing work on coreference. The five languages are all shown to allow gradable use of pronouns for entity and event coreference, with biases that align with existing generalizations about the link between prominence and the use of reduced referring expressions. The studies also show the feasibility of manipulating targeted verb-driven cues across multiple languages to support crosslingual comparisons.

2007 ◽  
Vol 48 ◽  
pp. 39-51 ◽  
Author(s):  
Peter Bosch ◽  
Carla Umbach

This paper discusses results from a corpus study of German demonstrative and personal pronouns and from a reading time experiment in which we compared the interpretation options of the two types of pronouns (Bosch et al. 2003, 2007). A careful review of exceptions to a generalisation we had been suggesting in those papers (the Subject Hypothesis: "Personal pronouns prefer subject antecedents and demonstratives prefer non-subject antecedents") shows that, although this generalisation correctly describes a tendency in the data, it is quite wrong in claiming that the grammatical role of antecedents is the relevant parameter. In the current paper we argue that the generalisation should be formulated in terms of in-formation-structural properties of referents rather than in terms of the grammatical role of antecedent expressions.  


1986 ◽  
Vol 24 ◽  
pp. 47-58 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gerard W. Bol ◽  
Folkert Kuiken

In this article we discuss the sequence of emergence of six different types of pronouns in 36 Dutch children from one to four. This study is part of a larger project concerning the morphosyntactic development in normal Dutch children of that age. The aim of this project is to find out whether or not there are patterns in the language of three different groups of language impaired children compared to the non-language impaired children. The main conclusions of this article are: the number of pronouns increases as the child gets older. Dutch children make very few mistakes in producing the pronouns studied. The sequence of emergence of interrogative pronouns reflects the order which is found in the English literature on the subject. Demonstrative pronouns are the first to emerge in the system, followed by personal pronouns. The subject forms emerge before the object forms. There is a clear tendency for singular pronouns to emerge before plurals.


2011 ◽  
Vol 33 (4) ◽  
pp. 691-724 ◽  
Author(s):  
ANNA L. THEAKSTON

ABSTRACTIn this study, 5-year-olds and adults described scenes that differed according to whether (a) the subject or object of a transitive verb represented an accessible or inaccessible referent, consistent or inconsistent with patterns of preferred argument structure, and (b) a simple noun was sufficient to uniquely identify an inaccessible referent. Results showed that although adults did not differ in their choice of referring expression based on sentence position, 5-year-olds were less likely to provide informative referring expressions for subjects than for objects when the referent was inaccessible. In addition, under complex discourse conditions, although adults used complex noun phrases to identify inaccessible referents, 5-year-olds increased their use of pronominal/null reference for both accessible and inaccessible referents, thus reducing their levels of informativeness. The data suggest that 5-year-olds are still learning to integrate their knowledge of discourse features with preferred argument structure patterns, that this is particularly difficult in complex discourse contexts, and that in these contexts children rely on well-rehearsed patterns of argument realization.


2017 ◽  
Vol 8 (1) ◽  
pp. 75 ◽  
Author(s):  
Doc. Dr. Jelena Š. Novaković ◽  
PhD student Božana Tomić

Apart from personal pronouns which are by far the most used referring expressions in English and Serbian, reference can be established and maintained using demonstratives.Their function is to refer to the location or distance of a person or an object. The aim of this paper is to examine reference realised by demonstratives with special regard to the restrictions written discourse imposes on their usage. The texts we used for analysis are narrative stories written in the two languages.


Author(s):  
András Bárány

This chapter turns to object agreement with personal pronouns in Hungarian. Pronouns are interesting because they do not always trigger agreement with the verb: first person objects never trigger object agreement (morphology), and second person pronouns only do with first person singular subjects. It is proposed that the distribution of object agreement is a morphological effect and argues that all personal pronouns do in fact trigger agreement, but agreement is not always spelled out. This means that Hungarian has an inverse agreement system, where the spell-out of agreement is determined by the relative person feature (or person feature sets) of the subject and the object. A formally explicit analysis of the syntax and the morphological spell-out of agreement is provided.


2020 ◽  
Vol 98 (Supplement_4) ◽  
pp. 140-143
Author(s):  
Roger E Lyons

Abstract A performance genetics model adapted to real-world data of Thoroughbred racing and breeding will be presented. The continuity of ancestry and performance is documented in the annals of pedigree, not as functional counterparts, but as corresponding signs of market value. Pedigree invites chronic misprision of highly diverse genetic resources competing for scarce opportunity in the racing economy, resulting in samples that are often small and of dubious composition. Data is specialized for competing “pedigree analysis” that packages the system of signification to meet market demand for meaning. Given statistical deficits, this data is a necessary inferential asset of the model. The model’s premise is that predictability is optimized if racing performance is defined as the function of an indivisible relation between parents. Statistical data consists of 6-generation ancestries of mares that produced offspring by a subject stallion. Comparison of proportions is used to identify effects resulting from his relation to individual ancestors of the subject mare. Expected performance by the sire’s offspring is defined as the proportion of mares that produced a superior runner by him. Each ancestor of a subject mare also has descendants among the mares that produced offspring by the stallion. For each of those groups, the proportion of mares that produced a superior runner is compared with the stallion’s expected performance using a t-test of statistical significance at the .10 level. Probable effect is further tested by case study involving such variables as racing class, generational distance, sex-linkage, inbreeding, and an ancestor’s pattern of effect across the stallion population. Stallions with the highest prevalence of positive effects are preferred for the subject mare. This model, under the trade name LyonScore®, has been used since 2012 by Werk Thoroughbred Constultants, Inc. as a component of its client services. Table 1: The data, listed in tabular format below, is graphically displayed for actual use on an ancestry tree whose nodes are numbered by relation to a subject mare (“Position”). Each statistical data item in the table is derived from the stud record of a stallion named Distorted Humor and corresponds with an ancestor of a mare named Positively Royal. Each ancestor of this mare is also an ancestor of a group of mares that produced offspring of Distorted Humor and were at least three years of age as of 2019. A proportion of each of those groups of mares produced at least one superior runner. Proportions that differ significantly from Distorted Humor’s expected proportion are so indicated. Only ancestors involving a group of at least 18 mares are considered to have inferential value on statistical grounds. Table 2: The sire Danzig is an ancestor of Positively Royal, along with 75 mares that produced foals by Distorted Humor, only five of which produced a superior runner by him, significantly fewer than expected. However, since Distorted Humor’s dam is by Danzig, the question of generational distance is relevant as a variable to the effect of inbreeding. The table below shows Danzig’s proportional distribution by genetic relation to those 75 mares as indicated by “Position.” Of the 64 mares in descent of Danzig within three generations, three mares produced a superior runner. It’s notable, though, that 2 of 6 mares with the same relation to Danzig as Positively Royal produced superior runners. Since Danzig is the only ancestor with a negative effect, further consideration is warranted. Table 3: In a population that tends to slough off unprofitable genetic resources, overspecialization is the main risk of close inbreeding. Some generational variations of an ancestor’s contribution can turn inbreeding to less specialized effect, but this depends on generational distance. Distorted Humor’s earliest opportunity with mares in descent of Danzig involved offspring closely inbred to Danzig, but later in his career he encountered mares for which variation was more likely. As the table shows, two of six mares descending from Danzig in position 25 (4th generation) produced Distorted Humor’s best runners inbred to Danzig, so it is of some interest that Positively Royal, the subject mare, is also a postion-25 descendant of Danzig. However, that those two mares happen to be full sisters whose breeding has little else in common with that of Positively Royal leaves the question of Distorted Humor’s fitness for this mare less certain than would be preferred.


1964 ◽  
Vol 42 (4) ◽  
pp. 575-582 ◽  
Author(s):  
James R. Wait

The concept that the concentric region between the earth and the ionosphere acts as a cavity resonator was proposed by Schumann over a decade ago. It is the purpose of this paper to review the theory of these cavity resonances. Some of the assumptions used in current work on the subject are also discussed. An alternative derivation is presented which appears to be more general than any given heretofore.


1989 ◽  
Vol 25 ◽  
pp. 167-191
Author(s):  
Eleonore Stump

Recent work on the subject of faith has tended to focus on the epistemology of religious belief, considering such issues as whether beliefs held in faith are rational and how they may be justified. Richard Swinburne, for example, has developed an intricate explanation of the relationship between the propositions of faith and the evidence for them. Alvin Plantinga, on the other hand, has maintained that belief in God may be properly basic, that is, that a belief that God exists can be part of the foundation of a rational noetic structure. This sort of work has been useful in drawing attention to significant issues in the epistemology of religion, but these approaches to faith seem to me also to deepen some long-standing perplexities about traditional Christian views of faith.


Author(s):  
D C Hesterman ◽  
B J Stone

It has been known for some time that the torsional vibration of reciprocating engines and pumps cannot be modelled accurately by representing the reciprocating mechanism by a constant inertia. There have been many publications describing better models than those that use constant inertia and these indicate that the effective inertia of a reciprocating mechanism varies with angular position. The major component of this variation is a twice per revolution cyclic effect—hence the term ‘secondary inertia’. The consequences of this secondary inertia effect can be serious for torsional vibration causing ‘secondary resonance,’ and even instability. This paper contains a review of the current literature on the subject and introduces some recent work by the authors.


2002 ◽  
Vol 27 ◽  
pp. 67-90 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ulrike Demske

Recent work on argument selection couched in a lexical decomposition approach (Ehrich & Rapp 2000) postulates different linking properties for verbs and nouns, challenging current views on argument inheritance. In this paper, I show that the different behavior with respect to verbal and nominal linking observed for Present-Day German does not carry over to ung-nominals in Early New High German. Deverbal nouns and corresponding verbs rather behave alike with respect to argument linking. I shall argue that this change is motivated by the growing rift between ung-nominals and their verbal bases both focussing on different parts oftheir lexicosemantic structure in Present-Day German. Evidence for the verb-like behavior of ung-nominals in Early New High German comes from the regular meaning relation between verbs and corresponding derived nouns, the actional properties of event-denoting nouns, and the patterning of ung-nominals with nominalized infinitives. Even their syntactic behavior reflects the verbal character of ung-nominals during that period of the German language. The diachronic facts can be accounted for in a straightforward way once we adopt a lexical decomposition approach to argument selection.  


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document