bearing angle
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

65
(FIVE YEARS 22)

H-INDEX

8
(FIVE YEARS 2)

2022 ◽  
Vol 18 (1) ◽  
pp. e1009772
Author(s):  
Marina Papadopoulou ◽  
Hanno Hildenbrandt ◽  
Daniel W. E. Sankey ◽  
Steven J. Portugal ◽  
Charlotte K. Hemelrijk

Bird flocks under predation demonstrate complex patterns of collective escape. These patterns may emerge by self-organization from local interactions among group-members. Computational models have been shown to be valuable for identifying what behavioral rules may govern such interactions among individuals during collective motion. However, our knowledge of such rules for collective escape is limited by the lack of quantitative data on bird flocks under predation in the field. In the present study, we analyze the first GPS trajectories of pigeons in airborne flocks attacked by a robotic falcon in order to build a species-specific model of collective escape. We use our model to examine a recently identified distance-dependent pattern of collective behavior: the closer the prey is to the predator, the higher the frequency with which flock members turn away from it. We first extract from the empirical data of pigeon flocks the characteristics of their shape and internal structure (bearing angle and distance to nearest neighbors). Combining these with information on their coordination from the literature, we build an agent-based model adjusted to pigeons’ collective escape. We show that the pattern of turning away from the predator with increased frequency when the predator is closer arises without prey prioritizing escape when the predator is near. Instead, it emerges through self-organization from a behavioral rule to avoid the predator independently of their distance to it. During this self-organization process, we show how flock members increase their consensus over which direction to escape and turn collectively as the predator gets closer. Our results suggest that coordination among flock members, combined with simple escape rules, reduces the cognitive costs of tracking the predator while flocking. Such escape rules that are independent of the distance to the predator can now be investigated in other species. Our study showcases the important role of computational models in the interpretation of empirical findings of collective behavior.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Marina Papadopoulou ◽  
Hanno Hildenbrandt ◽  
Daniel W.E. Sankey ◽  
Steven J. Portugal ◽  
Charlotte K. Hemelrijk

Bird flocks under predation demonstrate complex patterns of collective escape. These patterns may emerge by self-organization from simple interactions among group-members. Computational models have been shown to be valuable for identifying the behavioral rules that may govern these interactions among individuals during collective motion. However, our knowledge of such rules for collective escape is limited by the lack of quantitative data on bird flocks under predation in the field. In the present study, we analyze the first dataset of GPS trajectories of pigeons in airborne flocks attacked by a robotic falcon in order to build a species-specific model of collective escape. We use our model to examine a recently identified distance-dependent pattern of collective behavior that shows an increase in the escape frequency of pigeons when the predator is closer. We first extract from the empirical data the characteristics of pigeon flocks regarding their shape and internal structure (bearing angle and distance to nearest neighbours). Combining these with information on their coordination from the literature, we build an agent-based model tuned to pigeons' collective escape. We show that the pattern of increased escape frequency closer to the predator arises without flock-members prioritizing escape when the predator is near. Instead, it emerges through self-organization from an individual rule of predator-avoidance that is independent of predator-prey distance. During this self-organization process, we uncover a role of hysteresis and show that flock members increase their consensus over the escape direction and turn collectively as the predator gets closer. Our results suggest that coordination among flock-members, combined with simple escape rules, reduces the cognitive costs of tracking the predator. Such rules that are independent of predator-prey distance can now be examined in other species. Finally, we emphasize on the important role of computational models in the interpretation of empirical findings of collective behavior.


2021 ◽  
pp. 174702182110074
Author(s):  
Huaiyong Zhao ◽  
Dominik Straub ◽  
Constantin A. Rothkopf

Which strategy people use to guide locomotor interception remains unclear despite considerable research and the importance of an answer with ramification into the heuristics and biases debate. Because the constant bearing (CB) strategy corresponds to the target-heading (CTH) strategy with an additional constraint, these two strategies can be confounded experimentally. But, the two strategies are distinct in the information they require: while the CTH strategy only requires access to the relative angle between the direction of motion and the target, the CB strategy requires access to a stable allocentric reference frame. Here, we manipulated the visual information about allocentric reference frames in three virtual environments and asked participants to steer a car to intercept a moving target. Participants’ interception paths showed different degrees of curvature and their target-heading angles were approximately constant, consistent with the CTH strategy. By contrast, the target’s bearing angle continuously changed in all participants except one. This particular participant produced linear interception paths with little change in the target’s bearing angle, seemingly consistent with both strategies. This participant continued this pattern of steering even in the environment without any visual information about allocentric reference frames. Therefore, this pattern of steering is attributed to the CTH strategy rather than the CB strategy. The overall results add important evidence for the conclusion that locomotor interception is better accounted for by the CTH strategy and that experimentally observing a straight interception trajectory with a constant bearing angle is not sufficient evidence for the CB strategy.


2021 ◽  
Vol 12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gwenaelle Ceyte ◽  
Remy Casanova ◽  
Reinoud J. Bootsma

Here we studied how participants steer to intercept uniformly moving targets in a virtual driving task. We tested the hypothesis that locomotor interception behavior cannot fully be explained by a strategy of nulling rate of change in pertinent agent-target relations such as the target-heading angle or target’s bearing angle. In line with a previously reported observation and model simulations, we found that, under specific combinations of initial target eccentricity and target motion direction, locomotor paths revealed reversals in movement direction. This phenomenon is not compatible with unique reliance on first-order (i.e., rate-of-change based) information in the case of uniformly moving targets. We also found that, as expected, such reversals in movement direction were not observed consistently over all trials of the same experimental condition: their presence depended on the timing of the first steering action effected by the participant, with only early steering actions leading to reversals in movement direction. These particular characteristics of the direction-reversal phenomenon demonstrated here for a locomotor interception-by-steering task correspond to those reported for lateral manual interception. Together, these findings suggest that control strategies operating in manual and locomotor interception may at least share certain characteristics.


2021 ◽  
pp. 1-1
Author(s):  
Chien-Chou Lin ◽  
Chih-Hung Kuo ◽  
Hsin-Te Chiang
Keyword(s):  

2021 ◽  
Vol 46 (2) ◽  
Author(s):  
A. V. Volovyk ◽  
◽  
A. V. Naumenko

The paper considers the problem of detecting the position of unmanned aerial vehicles, which are equipped with appropriate equipment for unauthorized receipt of confidential information at the object of information activities. The Rao-Cramer boundary was used to determine the bearing angle of spy unmanned aerial vehicles using annular, arcuate and similar antenna arrays for radio direction finding using the maximum likelihood method and Music. Rao-Cramer limits are calculated for the location of signals in azimuth and for the signal-to-noise ratio. The dependence of the standard deviation for azimuth and angular direction finding in the presence of noise is obtained.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Oan Chul Choi ◽  
Byoung Kook Kim ◽  
Siyoung Jung ◽  
Hajin Choi

PLoS ONE ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 15 (12) ◽  
pp. e0241280
Author(s):  
Daniel García Párraga ◽  
Peter L. Tyack ◽  
Vicente Marco-Cabedo ◽  
José Luis Crespo-Picazo ◽  
Xavier Manteca ◽  
...  

Homing pigeons (Columba livia domestica) were used to test whether clinical magnetic resonance (MR) imaging disrupts orientation of animals that sense the earth’s magnetic field. Thirty young pigeons were randomly separated into three groups (n = 10/group). Two groups were anaesthetized and exposed to either a constant (no sequence) or a varying (gradient echo and echo planar sequences) magnetic field within a 3 Tesla MR unit for 15 minutes. The control group was not exposed to the MR field but shared all other aspects of the procedure. One day later, animals were released from a site they had never visited, 15 km from the home loft. Three weeks after the procedure, animals were released from a different unfamiliar site 30 km from the loft. Measured variables included the time to disappear from sight (seconds), vanishing bearing (angle), and the time interval from release to entering the home loft (hours). On first release, the group exposed to varying field gradients during image acquisition using 2 different standard sequences showed more variability in the vanishing bearing compared to the other groups (p = 0.0003 compared to control group), suggesting interference with orientation. Other measures did not show significant differences between groups. On second release, there were no significant differences between groups. Our results on homing pigeons show that regular clinical MR imaging exposure may temporarily affect the orientation of species that have magnetoreception capabilities. If exposure to MR imaging disrupted processes that are not specific to magnetoreception, then it may affect other species and other capabilities as well.


2020 ◽  
Vol 10 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Leandre Varennes ◽  
Holger G. Krapp ◽  
Stephane Viollet

AbstractEffective visuomotor coordination is a necessary requirement for the survival of many terrestrial, aquatic, and aerial animal species. We studied the kinematics of aerial pursuit in the blowfly Lucilia sericata using an actuated dummy as target for freely flying males. We found that the flies perform target tracking in the horizontal plane and target interception in the vertical plane. Our behavioural data suggest that the flies’ trajectory changes are a controlled combination of target heading angle and of the rate of change of the bearing angle. We implemented control laws in kinematic models and found that the contributions of proportional navigation strategy are negligible. We concluded that the difference between horizontal and vertical control relates to the difference in target heading angle the fly keeps constant: 0° in azimuth and 23° in elevation. Our work suggests that male Lucilia control both horizontal and vertical steerings by employing proportional controllers to the error angles. In horizontal plane, this controller operates at time delays as small as 10 ms, the fastest steering response observed in any flying animal, so far.


Author(s):  
Jason N. Greenberg ◽  
Xiaobo Tan

Abstract Localization of mobile robots is essential for navigation and data collection. This work presents an optical localization scheme for mobile robots during the robot’s continuous movement, despite that only one bearing angle can be captured at a time. In particular, this paper significantly improves upon our previous works where the robot has to pause its movement in order to acquire the two bearing angle measurements needed for position determination. The latter restriction forces the robot to work in a stop-and-go mode, which constrains the robot’s mobilitty. The proposed scheme exploits the velocity prediction from Kalman filtering, to properly correlate two consecutive measurements of bearing angles with respect to the base nodes (beacons) to produce location measurement. The proposed solution is evaluated in simulation and its advantage is demonstrated through the comparison with the traditional approach where the two consecutive angle measurements are directly used to compute the location.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document