attributional bias
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

86
(FIVE YEARS 11)

H-INDEX

22
(FIVE YEARS 1)

Author(s):  
Léa Decombe ◽  
Audrey Henry ◽  
René Decombe ◽  
Mélissa Tir ◽  
Anne Doé de Maindreville ◽  
...  

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Rebecca Peretz-Lange ◽  
Paul Muentener

Adults from Western cultures attribute others’ behavior to personal causes more readily than situational causes; however, little research has explored the developmental origins of this attributional bias. Research has shown that children can use both the statistical patterns present in observed behavior, as well as the verbal framing of the behaviors, to infer personal causes. However, research has not explored whether children also use these factors to infer situational causes. The present study examined the impacts of statistical patterns and verbal framing on four- and six-year-old children’s (n = 218) attributions to personal and situational causes for behavior, as assessed by their explanations for characters’ interactions with toys. In a factorial design the statistical pattern of characters’ behaviors suggested either a personal or situational cause (or neither), and the experimenter’s verbal framing of the behaviors suggested either a personal or situational cause (or neither). Across age groups, children showed a bias toward providing personal explanations. Both statistical pattern and verbal framing influenced causal attributions, but both impacts were asymmetric such that situational cues increased situational explanations relative to neutral cues, but there was no difference in children’s explanations following personal and neutral cues. These results suggest that verbal framing and statistical patterns impact children’s developing social causal attributions, specifically with respect to situational causes, and also that a personal attribution bias emerges early in development.


2021 ◽  
Vol 7 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Parola Alberto ◽  
Claudio Brasso ◽  
Rosalba Morese ◽  
Paola Rocca ◽  
Francesca M. Bosco

AbstractPatients with schizophrenia (SCZ) have a core impairment in the communicative-pragmatic domain, characterized by severe difficulties in correctly inferring the speaker’s communicative intentions. While several studies have investigated pragmatic performance of patients with SCZ, little research has analyzed the errors committed in the comprehension of different communicative acts. The present research investigated error patterns in 24 patients with SCZ and 24 healthy controls (HC) during a task assessing the comprehension of different communicative acts, i.e., sincere, deceitful and ironic, and their relationship with the clinical features of SCZ. We used signal detection analysis to quantify participants’ ability to correctly detect the speakers’ communicative intention, i.e., sensitivity, and their tendency to wrongly perceive a communicative intention when not present, i.e., response bias. Further, we investigated the relationship between sensitivity and response bias, and the clinical features of the disorder, namely symptom severity, pharmacotherapy, and personal and social functioning. The results showed that the ability to infer the speaker’s communicative intention is impaired in SCZ, as patients exhibited lower sensitivity, compared to HC, for all the pragmatic phenomena evaluated, i.e., sincere, deceitful, and ironic communicative acts. Further, we found that the sensitivity measure for irony was related to disorganized/concrete symptoms. Moreover, patients with SCZ showed a stronger response bias for deceitful communicative acts compared to HC: when committing errors, they tended to misattribute deceitful intentions more often than sincere and ironic ones. This tendency to misattribute deceitful communicative intentions may be related to the attributional bias characterizing the disorder.


2021 ◽  
pp. 088626052198984
Author(s):  
Yun Zeng ◽  
Xilin Liu ◽  
Lehua Cheng

Emotion perception has a vital influence on social interaction. Previous studies discussed mainly the relationship between facial emotion perception and aggressive behavior from the perspective of hostile attributional bias and the impaired violence inhibition mechanism. The present study aims to provide new evidence of different emotion perception patterns between the violent and non-violent criminal samples through a new indicator of the facial emotion recognition test, Facial Emotion Perception Tendency (FEPT), calculated by counting the times a participant recognizes a set of emotional stimuli as a particular specific emotion, and to further examine the association between aggressive behaviors and FEPT. 101 violent and 171 non-violent offenders, as well as 81 non-offending control participants, were recruited to complete the emotion recognition task with morphed stimuli (Study 1). We further recruited 62 non-offending healthy male participants to finish the Buss -Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ) after the emotion recognition task in Study 2. Both non-violent and violent offenders were significantly lower in overall accuracy of emotion recognition and disgust FEPT, but higher in happy FEPT, than non-offending healthy controls. Non-violent offenders had significantly lower fear FEPT than violent offenders, and had higher anger FEPT than non-offending controls. The results also revealed that the level of physical aggression was positively correlated with fear FEPT, while negatively correlated with anger FEPT. The current study demonstrated that FEPT was associated with aggressive behavior and implies the importance of improving the emotion decoding ability of offenders. Also, the concept “FEPT” proposed in this study is of significance for further exploration of how individuals’ tendency to perceiving a particular emotion can be correlated with social behaviors.


2020 ◽  
pp. 216770262095152
Author(s):  
Philipp Riedel ◽  
William P. Horan ◽  
Junghee Lee ◽  
Gerhard S. Hellemann ◽  
Michael F. Green

Social cognition has become a major focus in psychosis research aimed at explaining heterogeneity in functional outcome and developing interventions oriented to functional recovery. However, there is still no consensus on the structure of social cognition in psychosis, and research in this area has been plagued by lack of replication. Our first goal was to replicate the factor structure of social cognition using nearly identical tasks in independent samples. Our second goal was to externally validate the factors as they relate to nonsocial cognition and various symptoms in the prediction of functioning using machine learning. Confirmatory factor analyses validated a three-factor model for social cognition in psychosis (low-level, high-level, attributional bias factor). A least absolute shrinkage and selection operator regression and cross-validation provided evidence for external validity of data-driven linear models including the social-cognitive factors, nonsocial cognition, and symptoms. We addressed the replicability problems that have impeded research in this area, and our results will guide future psychosis studies.


2020 ◽  
Vol 129 ◽  
pp. 176-180 ◽  
Author(s):  
Arthur Pabst ◽  
Elodie Peyroux ◽  
Benjamin Rolland ◽  
Philippe de Timary ◽  
Pierre Maurage

2020 ◽  
Vol 64 (13-14) ◽  
pp. 1443-1460
Author(s):  
Parwinder Singh

Aggression is a multidimensional phenomenon, and for its better understanding, specificity involved in its dynamics must be explored. This article explores the role of attributional bias as a mediator between impulsivity and aggressive tendencies among adolescents. The mediating effect of hostile attributional bias (HAB) on the impulsivity–aggression relationship has not been studied extensively so far, especially in the Indian context. For testing the hypotheses, 320 participants within the age range 12 to 15 years ( M age = 13.57 years) were selected and administered relevant standardized questionnaires. Baron and Kenny’s criterion was used for mediation analysis, demonstrating that HAB partially mediates the association between impulsivity and aggression. Findings imply that reducing HAB through some reattribution intervention may be an effective strategy to reduce impulsivity-induced aggression. Specific guidelines to implement such interventions are suggested in the discussion.


2020 ◽  
Vol 46 (Supplement_1) ◽  
pp. S64-S65
Author(s):  
Covadonga Díaz-Caneja ◽  
Marcos González-Iglesias ◽  
Victoria Del Amo ◽  
Ignacio García-Cabeza ◽  
Celso Arango ◽  
...  

Abstract Background Deficits in social cognition could be involved in the pathogenesis of delusions in psychotic disorders (Bentall et al., 2009). Childhood trauma (CT) has been associated with an increased risk for psychosis (Varese et al., 2012). Neurocognitive and social cognition deficits could mediate in the association between CT and psychosis (Mansueto et al., 2019). Social cognition and childhood trauma have been understudied so far in delusional disorder (DD). We aimed to assess social cognition in a sample of patients with delusional psychoses (i.e., DD and schizophrenia) and healthy controls (HC) and to explore the potential effect of childhood trauma on social cognition and delusion. Methods This cross-sectional, transdiagnostic study included 69 patients with a DSM-IV-TR-confirmed diagnosis of DD (mean age 44.06 ± 11.39 years, 53.6% female), 77 with DSM-IV-TR-confirmed schizophrenia (mean age 38.12 ± 9.27 years, 27.3% female), and 63 HC (mean age 43.6 ± 13.0 years, 68.3% female). Attributional bias was assessed with the “Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire.” Theory of Mind (ToM) performance was assessed with the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test” and the “Faux Pas Recognition Test.” Childhood trauma was measured with the “Childhood Trauma Questionnaire.” Neuropsychological functioning was measured with a comprehensive battery assessing attention, verbal learning, working memory, and executive function. We used ANCOVAs and linear regression analyses to assess the association between the three measures of social cognition and i) diagnosis, ii) dimensional measures of delusion proneness (Peters Delusion Inventory, PDI) and intensity (Maudsley Assessment of Delusion Schedule, MADS), and iii) childhood trauma; after controlling for potential confounders (age, sex, socioeconomic status, and estimated premorbid intelligence quotient). Results Patients with DD showed significantly poorer performance on the “Eyes Test” than HC (Cohen’s d=-0.44, p=0.037), after controlling for potential confounding variables. The difference was no longer significant after controlling for verbal memory. Patients with schizophrenia (d=-1.54, p<0.001) and DD (Cohen’s d=-0.60, p=0.002) showed significantly poorer performance than HC on the “Faux Pas Test,” after controlling for potential confounders. The difference between patients with schizophrenia and HC remained significant after controlling for neuropsychological functioning (Cohen’s d=-1.09, p<0.001), while differences between patients with DD and HC were no longer significant after controlling for executive function and working memory performance (Cohen’s d=-0.23, p=0.596). No significant differences were found between diagnostic groups in externalizing or personalizing attributional bias. In the fully adjusted models, intensity of the delusional idea was significantly associated with performance in the “Faux Pas Test” in DD, and with externalizing and personalizing attributional bias in schizophrenia. A positive history of CT was significantly associated with lower performance on the “Faux Pas Test” (Cohen’s d=-0.40, p=.022) and higher delusional proneness scores in the delusional psychosis samples (Cohen’s d=-0.49, p=.006), but not in HC. Discussion Social cognition deficits are associated with delusional intensity in delusional psychoses. Childhood trauma could increase the risk of psychosis through its effect on social cognition.


2019 ◽  
Vol 16 (3) ◽  
pp. 185-192
Author(s):  
You Jin Park ◽  
Jin Young Park ◽  
Kyung-Mi Chung ◽  
Yul-Mai Song ◽  
Kyungun Jhung

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document