Influence of the mean luminance on the detection threshold for red-green chromatic gratings

2002 ◽  
Author(s):  
Juan L. Nieves Gomez ◽  
E. Valero ◽  
Javier Hernandez-Andres ◽  
Jose A. Garcia ◽  
Javier Romero
1983 ◽  
Vol 245 (5) ◽  
pp. R637-R641 ◽  
Author(s):  
J. V. Odom ◽  
N. M. Bromberg ◽  
W. W. Dawson

The visual acuities (i.e., visual thresholds for pattern detection) of four dogs under neuromuscular block were measured using visually evoked cortical potentials (VECP) and/or pattern-evoked retinal potentials (PERR). Stimuli were phase-reversing square-wave gratings with a mean luminance of 86 cd/m2 and 70% contrast. The mean of the VECP thresholds of two dogs tested was 12.59 cycles per degree (cycles/deg). The mean of the PERR thresholds of four dogs tested was 11.61 cycles/deg. The difference between VECP and PERR thresholds was not statistically significant. VECP acuities appear to be determined at or before the last stage of retinal processing (PERR). Our estimates of canine acuity are 1.3–2 times those reported for cats and 0.2–0.4 times those reported for primates when tested under comparable luminance and contrast conditions.


1996 ◽  
Vol 107 (1) ◽  
pp. 121-131 ◽  
Author(s):  
M Mizunami

Synaptic transmission from second- to third-order neurons of cockroach ocelli occurs in an exponentially rising part of the overall sigmoidal characteristic curve relating pre- and postsynaptic voltage. Because of the nonlinear nature of the synapse, linear responses of second-order neurons to changes in ligh intensity are half-wave rectified, i.e., the response to a decrement in light is amplified whereas that to an increment in light is compressed. Here I report that the gain of synaptic transmission from second- to third-order neurons changes by ambient light levels and by wind stimulation applied to the cerci. Transfer characteristics of the synapse were studied by simultaneous intracellular recordings of second- and third-order neurons. Potential changes were evoked in second-order neurons by a sinusoidally modulated light with various mean luminances. With a decrease in the mean luminance (a) the mean membrane potential of second-order neurons was depolarized, (b) the synapse between the second- and third-order neurons operated in a steeper range of the exponential characteristic curve, where the gain to transmit modulatory signals was higher, and (c) the gain of third-order neurons to detect a decrement in light increased. Second-order neurons were depolarized when a wind or tactile stimulus was applied to various parts of the body including the cerci. During a wind-evoked depolarization, the synapse operated in a steeper range of the characteristic curve, which resulted in an increased gain of third-order neurons to detect light decrements. I conclude that the nonlinear nature of the synapse between the second- and third-order neurons provides an opportunity for an adjustment of gain to transmit signals of intensity change. The possibility that a similar gain control occurs in other visual systems and underlies a more advanced visual function, i.e., detection of motion, is discussed.


1994 ◽  
Vol 8 (5) ◽  
pp. 259-264 ◽  
Author(s):  
Shigeru Furuta ◽  
Kengo Nishimoto ◽  
Masahiko Egawa ◽  
Masaru Ohyama ◽  
Hiroyuki Moriyama

We studied the olfactory functions of organic mercury poisoning (Minamata disease) by using both the smell identification test and the olfactory detection threshold test (with phenyl ethyl alcohol). The subjects were 19 patients with Minamata disease who were treated in Meisuien, Minamata, Kumamoto, Japan and include cases that developed the disease in utero. The mean age was 78.7 ± 14.3 years old. Both smell identification and olfactory detection tests in the majority of patients decreased significantly compared with those of healthy elder subjects. A few cases showed normal olfactory identification and detection functions. The olfactory identification function deceased with advancing age. Correlation existed between detection threshold and background factors such as age and duration after documented Minamata disease was found. No significant relationship was identified between olfactory function and the typical symptoms of Minamata disease.


2005 ◽  
Vol 125 (6) ◽  
pp. 641-660 ◽  
Author(s):  
David Holcman ◽  
Juan I. Korenbrot

Detection threshold in cone photoreceptors requires the simultaneous absorption of several photons because single photon photocurrent is small in amplitude and does not exceed intrinsic fluctuations in the outer segment dark current (dark noise). To understand the mechanisms that limit light sensitivity, we characterized the molecular origin of dark noise in intact, isolated bass single cones. Dark noise is caused by continuous fluctuations in the cytoplasmic concentrations of both cGMP and Ca2+ that arise from the activity in darkness of both guanylate cyclase (GC), the enzyme that synthesizes cGMP, and phosphodiesterase (PDE), the enzyme that hydrolyzes it. In cones loaded with high concentration Ca2+ buffering agents, we demonstrate that variation in cGMP levels arise from fluctuations in the mean PDE enzymatic activity. The rates of PDE activation and inactivation determine the quantitative characteristics of the dark noise power density spectrum. We developed a mathematical model based on the dynamics of PDE activity that accurately predicts this power spectrum. Analysis of the experimental data with the theoretical model allows us to determine the rates of PDE activation and deactivation in the intact photoreceptor. In fish cones, the mean lifetime of active PDE at room temperature is ∼55 ms. In nonmammalian rods, in contrast, active PDE lifetime is ∼555 ms. This remarkable difference helps explain why cones are noisier than rods and why cone photocurrents are smaller in peak amplitude and faster in time course than those in rods. Both these features make cones less light sensitive than rods.


1990 ◽  
Vol 258 (3) ◽  
pp. R820-R826 ◽  
Author(s):  
A. C. Spector ◽  
G. J. Schwartz ◽  
H. J. Grill

Electrophysiological data support the existence of sodium-specific taste receptors that appear to be limited to the anterior tongue. However, previous behavioral findings suggest that bilateral transection of the chorda tympani nerve (CTn) has minimal consequences on NaCl intake and preference. This study employed a conditioned avoidance procedure to measure detection thresholds to NaCl and sucrose both before and after bilateral transection of the CTn. Rats were trained to maintain spout contact in response to water presentations (70 microliters) and to avoid spout contact when a taste solution (70 microliters) was presented. In experiment 1, all rats (n = 3) showed statistically significant impairments in the detectability of NaCl after bilateral section of the CTn. The mean increase in the NaCl detection threshold was 1.41 log units. In contrast, sucrose threshold in these same rats was marginally affected by CTn section (mean increase = 0.22 log units). Experiment 2 (n = 4) replicated the findings of the first experiment. The mean increase in the NaCl detection threshold was 2.23 log units. Sucrose threshold in these rats was, again, only marginally affected by CTn section (mean increase = 0.83 log units). Histological examination of the anterior tongue from the rats in experiment 2 indicated that the CTn transections were complete. These findings reveal that the anterior oral receptive field (innervated by the CTn) containing only 15% of the total taste bud population is critical for the normal detection of NaCl.


1975 ◽  
Vol 40 (1) ◽  
pp. 84-91 ◽  
Author(s):  
Randall C. Beattie ◽  
Dion V. Svihovec ◽  
Brad J. Edgerton

Using three speakers and 75 subjects, a 7.9-dB range in the relative intelligibility of the Central Institute for the Deaf (CID) spondees was found when they were presented via monitored live voice. This range is consistent with previous research and is considered unnecessarily large and detrimental to the efficiency and precision of the speech reception threshold (SRT) test. The exclusive use of 18 words that have a mean range of 1.5 dB is suggested. The slope of the 36-item spondee articulation function is 12% per decibel and is slightly steeper than previous research using the CID W-l recorded spondees. The slope for the suggested 18-item test is also about 12% per decibel. For the 36-item test the mean speech detection threshold (SDT) was 6.6 dB sound pressure level (SPL) and the mean SRT was 14.2 db SPL. The 7.6-dB difference between the two speech measures is consistent with previous research but the SRT is about 6 dB better than the 20 dB SPL recommended by ANSI 1969. For the suggested 18-item test, the mean SRT was 14.5 dB SPL, which was 7.9 dB above SDT. When the 18-item test is used, average normal hearing can be expected to be about 5.5 dB better than specified by ANSI 1969.


2019 ◽  
Vol 31 (2) ◽  
pp. 25-31
Author(s):  
Hassanien J. Mohammed ◽  
Taghreed F. Zaidan

Background: Hyperlipidemia is an elevated fat (lipids), mostly cholesterol and triglycerides, in the blood. These lipids usually bind to proteins to remain circulated so-called lipoprotein. Aims of the study: To determine taste detection threshold and estimate the trace elements (zinc) in serum and saliva of those patients and compare all of these with healthy control subjects. Methods: Eighty subjects were incorporated in this study, thy were divided into two groups: forty patients on simvastatin treatment age between (35-60) years, and forty healthy control of age range between (35-60) years. Saliva was collected by non-stimulated technique within 10 minutes. Serum was obtained from each subject. Zinc was estimated in serum and saliva by flame atomic absorption assay. Taste detection threshold was estimated by using 15 different concentrations of the four basic tastes solutions, the test use sip and spit with deionized water as mouth wash interval. Diabetics, thyroid and parathyroid disease, autoimmune disease, chemotherapy, smoking, alcoholics, neoplastic diseases were excluded. Results: The study showed that the taste detection threshold of sour and bitter were highly significantly higher in those patients than that in control subjects, sweet detection threshold were significantly high in patient on simvastatin. The salt detection threshold showed no significant differences between study groups. Salivary flow rate was significantly decreased in patients on simvastatin treatment than that in control subjects. Salivary and serum zinc were highly significantly decreased in control subjects than those in patients. There was highly significantly positive linear correlation between salivary flow rate and the mean of detection threshold of sweetness and sourness of both study groups, and highly significantly negative linear correlation with the mean of detection threshold of saltiness and bitterness in both study groups.


2020 ◽  
Vol 287 (1918) ◽  
pp. 20192537
Author(s):  
Ioan E. Smart ◽  
Innes C. Cuthill ◽  
Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel

Most animals need to move, and motion will generally break camouflage. In many instances, most of the visual field of a predator does not fall within a high-resolution area of the retina and so, when an undetected prey moves, that motion will often be in peripheral vision. We investigate how this can be exploited by prey, through different patterns of movement, to reduce the accuracy with which the predator can locate a cryptic prey item when it subsequently orients towards a target. The same logic applies for a prey species trying to localize a predatory threat. Using human participants as surrogate predators, tasked with localizing a target on peripherally viewed computer screens, we quantify the effects of movement (duration and speed) and target pattern. We show that, while motion is certainly detrimental to camouflage, should movement be necessary, some behaviours and surface patterns reduce that cost. Our data indicate that the phenotype that minimizes localization accuracy is unpatterned, having the mean luminance of the background, does not use a startle display prior to movement, and has short (below saccadic latency), fast movements.


1977 ◽  
Vol 44 (3_suppl) ◽  
pp. 1051-1056 ◽  
Author(s):  
Michael O'Grady

In a test of the hypothesis that a subliminally presented emotionally charged picture will elicit a greater deflection of skin resistance than a subliminally presented neutral picture, 24 subjects were stimulated subliminally at three standardized exposure times which were 3, 2, and 1 SDs shorter than their mean detection threshold. Mean deflections of skin resistance for 12 male and 12 female subjects, neutral and emotional stimuli, and the three exposure times were compared. The mean deflection for emotional stimuli was significantly larger than for neutral stimuli as hypothesized.


1985 ◽  
Vol 86 (3) ◽  
pp. 423-453 ◽  
Author(s):  
R L Chappell ◽  
K Naka ◽  
M Sakuranaga

The small- and large-field (cone) horizontal cells produce similar dynamic responses to a stimulus whose mean luminance is modulated by a white-noise signal. Nonlinear components increase with an increase in the mean luminance and may produce a mean square error (MSE) of up to 15%. Increases in the mean luminance of the field stimulus bring about three major changes: the incremental sensitivity defined by the amplitude of the kernels decreases in a Weber-Fechner fashion; the waveforms of the kernels are transformed from monophasic (integrating) to biphasic (differentiating); the peak response time of the kernels becomes shorter and the cells respond to much higher-frequency inputs. The dynamics of the horizontal cell response also depend on the area of the retina stimulated. Smaller spots of light produce monophasic kernels of a longer peak response time. The presence of a steady background produces three major changes in the spot kernels: the kernel's amplitude becomes larger (incremental sensitivity increases); the peak response times become shorter; the waveform of the kernels changes in a fashion similar to that observed with an increase in the mean luminance of the field stimulus. A similar enhancement in the incremental sensitivity by a steady background has also been observed in catfish, which shows that this phenomenon is a common feature of the horizontal cells in the lower vertebrate retina.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document