scholarly journals Perspective Cues Make Eye-specific Contributions to 3-D Motion Perception

2021 ◽  
pp. 1-17
Author(s):  
Lowell W. Thompson ◽  
Byounghoon Kim ◽  
Zikang Zhu ◽  
Bas Rokers ◽  
Ari Rosenberg

Abstract Robust 3-D visual perception is achieved by integrating stereoscopic and perspective cues. The canonical model describing the integration of these cues assumes that perspective signals sensed by the left and right eyes are indiscriminately pooled into a single representation that contributes to perception. Here, we show that this model fails to account for 3-D motion perception. We measured the sensitivity of male macaque monkeys to 3-D motion signaled by left-eye perspective cues, right-eye perspective cues, stereoscopic cues, and all three cues combined. The monkeys exhibited idiosyncratic differences in their biases and sensitivities for each cue, including left- and right-eye perspective cues, suggesting that the signals undergo at least partially separate neural processing. Importantly, sensitivity to combined cue stimuli was greater than predicted by the canonical model, which previous studies found to account for the perception of 3-D orientation in both humans and monkeys. Instead, 3-D motion sensitivity was best explained by a model in which stereoscopic cues were integrated with left- and right-eye perspective cues whose representations were at least partially independent. These results indicate that the integration of perspective and stereoscopic cues is a shared computational strategy across 3-D processing domains. However, they also reveal a fundamental difference in how left- and right-eye perspective signals are represented for 3-D orientation versus motion perception. This difference results in more effective use of available sensory information in the processing of 3-D motion than orientation and may reflect the temporal urgency of avoiding and intercepting moving objects.

Segmental sensory receptive fields in axolotl hindlimb skin were mapped during extracellular recording of nerve responses to light tactile stimulation. Normally, cutaneous sensory innervation patterns for a given pair of left and right hindlimbs were similar, but there was variability among animals. Individual cutaneous fibres innervated a solitary receptive field whose borders were sharply defined. When spinal nerves were crushed or cut and allowed to regrow the receptive fields re-established were similar to those on the normal contralateral limb. However, many single cutaneous fibres innervated multiple receptive fields. After cutting and interchanging the two major limb nerve branches, regenerating cutaneous nerves tended to innervate skin toward which they were directed, and receptive fields did not resemble the patterns on the control limb skin. This contrasts with the results following the same operations on the motor innervation where patterns of re-innervation do resemble the control. Regenerating cutaneous fibres apparently cannot relocate their respective original cutaneous addresses, but readily re-innervate foreign skin areas. Nerves regenerating after a crush or cut appear to follow mechanical and/or biochemical orienting clues within the nerve trunks for restoration of typical innervation patterns. It is not known how the axolotl central nervous system copes with cutaneous sensory information from mislocated nerve terminals.


2017 ◽  
Vol 284 (1858) ◽  
pp. 20170673 ◽  
Author(s):  
Irene Senna ◽  
Cesare V. Parise ◽  
Marc O. Ernst

Unlike vision, the mechanisms underlying auditory motion perception are poorly understood. Here we describe an auditory motion illusion revealing a novel cue to auditory speed perception: the temporal frequency of amplitude modulation (AM-frequency), typical for rattling sounds. Naturally, corrugated objects sliding across each other generate rattling sounds whose AM-frequency tends to directly correlate with speed. We found that AM-frequency modulates auditory speed perception in a highly systematic fashion: moving sounds with higher AM-frequency are perceived as moving faster than sounds with lower AM-frequency. Even more interestingly, sounds with higher AM-frequency also induce stronger motion aftereffects. This reveals the existence of specialized neural mechanisms for auditory motion perception, which are sensitive to AM-frequency. Thus, in spatial hearing, the brain successfully capitalizes on the AM-frequency of rattling sounds to estimate the speed of moving objects. This tightly parallels previous findings in motion vision, where spatio-temporal frequency of moving displays systematically affects both speed perception and the magnitude of the motion aftereffects. Such an analogy with vision suggests that motion detection may rely on canonical computations, with similar neural mechanisms shared across the different modalities.


2008 ◽  
Vol 25 (5-6) ◽  
pp. 675-684 ◽  
Author(s):  
CYNTHIA HALL-HARO ◽  
LYNNE KIORPES

AbstractWe studied the development of sensitivity to complex motion using plaid patterns. We hypothesized, based on neurophysiological data showing a dearth of pattern direction–selective (PDS) cells in area medial temporal (MT) of infant macaques, that sensitivity to pattern motion would develop later than other forms of global motion sensitivity. We tested 10 macaque monkeys (Macaca nemestrina) ranging in age from 7 weeks to 109–160 weeks (adult). The monkeys discriminated horizontal from vertical pattern motion; sensitivity for one-dimensional (1D) direction discrimination and detection were tested as control tasks. The results show that pattern motion discrimination ability develops relatively late, between 10 and 18 weeks, while performance on the 1D control tasks was excellent at the earliest test ages. Plaid discrimination performance depends on both the speed and spatial scale of the underlying patterns. However, development is not limited by contrast sensitivity. These results support the idea that pattern motion perception depends on a different mechanism than other forms of global motion perception and are consistent with the idea that the representation of PDS neurons in MT may limit the development of complex motion perception.


Robotica ◽  
1990 ◽  
Vol 8 (1) ◽  
pp. 7-12 ◽  
Author(s):  
C. R. Weisbin ◽  
B. L. Burks ◽  
J. R. Einstein ◽  
R. R. Feezell ◽  
W. W. Manges ◽  
...  

SUMMARYHERMIES-III is an autonomous robot comprised of a seven degree-of-freedom (DOF) manipulator designed for human scale tasks, a laser range finder, a sonar array, an omnidirectional wheel-driven chassis, multiple cameras, and a dual computer system containing a 16-node hypercube expandable to 128 nodes. The current experimental program involves performance of human-scale tasks (e.g., valve manipulation, use of tools), integration of a dexterous manipulator and platform motion in geometrically complex environments, and effective use of multiple cooperating robots (HERMIES-IIB and HERMIES-III). The environment in which the robots operate has been designed to include multiple valves, pipes, meters, obstacles on the floor, valves occluded from view, and multiple paths of differing navigation complexity. The ongoing research program supports the development of autonomous capability for HERMIES-IIB and III to perform complex navigation and manipulation under time constraints, while dealing with imprecise sensory information.


2017 ◽  
Vol 5 (5) ◽  
pp. 827-842 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kimberly B. Schauder ◽  
Woon Ju Park ◽  
Duje Tadin ◽  
Loisa Bennetto

Atypical visual motion perception has been widely observed in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The pattern of results, however, has been inconsistent. Emerging mechanistic hypotheses seek to explain these variable patterns of atypical motion sensitivity, each uniquely predicting specific patterns of performance across varying stimulus conditions. Here, we investigated the integrity of two such fundamental mechanisms—response gain control and receptive field size. A total of 20 children and adolescents with ASD and 20 typically developing (TD) age- and IQ-matched controls performed a motion discrimination task. To adequately model group differences in both mechanisms of interest, we tested a range of 23 stimulus conditions varying in size and contrast. Results revealed a motion perception impairment in ASD that was specific to the smallest sized stimuli (1°), irrespective of stimulus contrast. Model analyses provided evidence for larger receptive field size in ASD as the mechanism that explains this size-specific reduction of motion sensitivity.


Author(s):  
Mr. M. Senthil Murugan ◽  
Renuka E. ◽  
Vinodhini M.

One of the most critical subjects of embedded vision is color tracking in real time. Many computer vision applications begin by detecting and tracking moving objects in video scenes. Customers arriving at hypermarkets may benefit from this concept. A color detection algorithm locates pixels in an image that fit a predetermined color scheme. To differentiate detected pixels from the rest of the image, the color of the detected pixels can be modified. The robot is programmed to track objects by turning left and right to keep the target in view and driving forward and backward to keep the distance between the robot and the object steady. By maintaining a surrounding distance, detection of other objects of the same color pattern is ignored. By keeping a safe distance between the user and the robot, other objects of the same color pattern are not detected. The camera on an ARM11 Raspberry Pi computer attached to the robot is used to capture images. Using inbuilt python files, the acquired image is processed to locate the color using RGB varying pattern methodology. To make the product work smarter, this system also includes automatic billing via RFID reader and tag. The new concept of image processing domain is based on this device theory.


2020 ◽  
Vol 46 (Supplement_1) ◽  
pp. S209-S209
Author(s):  
Francina Badia ◽  
Daniel Linares ◽  
Albert Compte ◽  
Mireia Rosa ◽  
Josep Dalmau ◽  
...  

Abstract Background Perceptual spatial suppression is a phenomenon in which the perceived strength of a stimulus in space is reduced when the stimulus is surrounded by other stimuli. For motion perception, two studies so far have suggested that spatial suppression and sensitivity to motion perception is also reduced in patients with schizophrenia. Studies to date have been conducted in patients with chronic schizophrenia, however, whether these abnormalities are present at the onset of the disorder or whether they emerge during the course of the illness has not been examined, and no study has assessed whether these abnormalities are specific to schizophrenia or whether they are present in other psychotic disorders. Furthermore, if reduced spatial suppression and sensitivity for motion in schizophrenia are related to a glutamatergic hypofunction, as suggested by a recent study (Schallmo et al., 2019), these reductions may be more accentuated in patients who fail to respond to first-line antipsychotic treatment. Methods Sample: 33 patients with a first psychotic episode (16 females, age=16.4±0.6) and 17 healthy controls (9 females, age=17.2±0.61). Exclusion criteria for both groups were: intellectual disability according to DSM-V criteria. For healthy controls, exclusion criteria also included having a first degree relative with a history of psychotic disorder, current or past diagnosis of psychiatrics disorders. Instruments: The perceptual test was performed on a tablet, and consisted of a briefly presented grating (small or large) drifted sideways (the direction was chosen at random with equal probability), in which the participant was instructed to report the perceived direction. Clinical assessment at illness onset and 12 week follow-up: Positive and Negative Symptom Scales (PANSS), Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia, Present and Lifetime version and Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV. Non-response to treatment was defined as lack of 50% reduction in PANSS positive or negative scores at 12 weeks, any change in antipsychotics or need for combinations due to lack of clinical response. Psychophysical analysis: Motion sensitivity was estimated independently of lapses of attention, which were assessed by including trials in which the motion stimulus was easily discriminated. Results Patients and healthy controls were homogeneous in age (t=-.720, p=,537) and sex (X2=0.38, p=0.542). In patients, mean treatment response rates was 56.5%. Patients had similar scores of positive and negative symptomatology (positive symptoms= 21±7,13; negative symptoms= 18,4±8,18; general symptoms= 40,7±13,07). At 12 weeks 43,8% had a diagnosis of affective psychosis (bipolar disorder, depressive disorder with psychotic symptoms). Patients with a first psychotic episode, regardless of diagnosis or response to treatment, had less motion sensitivity than healthy controls (f=6.397, p=0.0148). No significant differences were found between groups in surround suppression and no significant correlations were observed between spatial suppression and clinical symptoms. Discussion To our knowledge, this is the first study to find abnormal motion sensitivity in patients with a first episode of psychosis. Our measure of sensitivity, given that it was not contaminated by lapses, indicates that patients had a genuine motion perception deficit rather than an inability to focus on the task. Our results also suggest that motion sensitivity may not be specific to patients with schizophrenia but may also characterize affective psychoses. Larger studies may be needed to clarify whether there is a relationship between motion sensitivity and severity of symptoms and response to treatment.


eLife ◽  
2014 ◽  
Vol 3 ◽  
Author(s):  
Julia L Semmelhack ◽  
Joseph C Donovan ◽  
Tod R Thiele ◽  
Enrico Kuehn ◽  
Eva Laurell ◽  
...  

Zebrafish larvae show characteristic prey capture behavior in response to small moving objects. The neural mechanism used to recognize objects as prey remains largely unknown. We devised a machine learning behavior classification system to quantify hunting kinematics in semi-restrained animals exposed to a range of virtual stimuli. Two-photon calcium imaging revealed a small visual area, AF7, that was activated specifically by the optimal prey stimulus. This pretectal region is innervated by two types of retinal ganglion cells, which also send collaterals to the optic tectum. Laser ablation of AF7 markedly reduced prey capture behavior. We identified neurons with arbors in AF7 and found that they projected to multiple sensory and premotor areas: the optic tectum, the nucleus of the medial longitudinal fasciculus (nMLF) and the hindbrain. These findings indicate that computations in the retina give rise to a visual stream which transforms sensory information into a directed prey capture response.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Joshua J Corbett

How do we perceive the location of moving objects? The position and motion literature is currently divided. Predictive accounts of object tracking propose that the position of moving objects is anticipated ahead of sensory signals, whilst non-predictive accounts claim that an anticipatory mechanism is not necessary. A novel illusion called the twinkle goes effect, describing a forward shift in the perceived final location of a moving object in the presence of dynamic noise, presents a novel opportunity to disambiguate these accounts. Across three experiments, we compared the predictions of predictive and non-predictive theories of object tracking by combining the twinkle goes paradigm with a multiple object tracking task. Specifically, we tested whether the size of the twinkle goes illusion would be smaller with greater attentional load (as entailed by the non-predictive, tracking continuation theory) or whether it would not be affected by attentional load (as entailed by predictive extrapolation theory). Our results failed to align with either of these theories of object localisation and tracking. Instead, we found evidence that the twinkle goes effect may be stronger with greater attentional load. We discuss whether this result may be a consequence of an essential, but previously unexplored relationship between the twinkle goes effect and representational momentum. In addition, this study was the first to reveal critical individual differences in the experience of the twinkle goes effect, and in the mislocalisation of moving objects. Together, our results continue to demonstrate the complexity of position and motion perception.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document