Capparis. Capparis cynophallophora . Above, flower, top view. Below, sterile branch. Drawn by P.N.Honychurch. Capparis flexuosa . a. Branch with flower buds, b. Leaf. c. Flower, side view. d. Flower longitudinally dissected, e. Fruit. Drawn by Priscilla Fawcett. From Correll and Correll 1982.

2004 ◽  
pp. 283-288
Keyword(s):  
Perception ◽  
1995 ◽  
Vol 24 (9) ◽  
pp. 1049-1058 ◽  
Author(s):  
Morton A Heller ◽  
John M Kennedy ◽  
Tamala D Joyner

Subjects were exposed to a three-dimensional model of a house and were asked to draw it using a raised-line drawing kit. Independent groups of ten each of sighted controls, early-blind, and late-blind subjects were told to identify the vantage point of tangible pictures of the model, including side views, ‘bird's-eye’ views from above, and views involving linear perspective. The ease or difficulty of picture interpretation depended upon the nature of the tangible drawing, with much better performance being recorded for side views. Performance was poor for foreshortened 3/4 views. Early-blind subjects were particularly unlikely to recognize views from above. In a control experiment with blindfolded sighted subjects the influence of prior information was examined: some subjects were told that the drawings could consist of side view or bird's-eye, top view, or 3/4 view drawings. This experiment showed that performance can be greatly improved through prior information about the nature of the tangible pictures.


2018 ◽  
Vol 208 ◽  
pp. 325-338 ◽  
Author(s):  
M. Ek ◽  
I. Beinik ◽  
A. Bruix ◽  
S. Wendt ◽  
J. V. Lauritsen ◽  
...  

Atomic arrangements in oxide surfaces can be uncovered by combining side view imaging using transmission electron microscopy and top view imaging using scanning tunnelling microscopy.


Author(s):  
M. Kessel ◽  
J.L. Gould-Kostka ◽  
D.L. Burns

We have characterised a protein from Bordetella pertussis, the whooping cough agent, and found it to be almost identical with the heat shock protein from E.coli, GroEL. These molecules, also termed chaperonins, are oligomeric proteins of approximately MW800.000 comprised of 14 subunits arranged in two rings of 7 subunits each, with a diameter of 15nm. When negatively stained, the GroEL-like molecules, examined in the electron microscope, are seen mainly in the top view clearly displaying the sevenfold symmetry, or as the less common side view with three characteristic electron dense striations separating four stain excluding regions (Fig. 1).


Author(s):  
Natalie R. Lodinger ◽  
Patricia R. DeLucia

Prior research on interruptions examined the effects of different characteristics of the primary and interrupting tasks on performance of the primary task. One measure is the resumption lag– the time between the end of the interrupting task and the next action in the resumed primary task (Altmann & Trafton, 2004). Prior research showed that an increase in the workload of a task results in an increase in resumption lag (Iqbal & Bailey, 2005). A common feature of prior studies of resumption lag is the use of computer-based tasks. However, interruptions occur in other types of tasks, such as laparoscopic surgery in which errors can result in serious consequences for the patient (Gillespie Chaboyer & Fairweather, 2012). Common interruptions during laparoscopic surgery include equipment failures and communication with team members (e.g., Gillespie et al.,2012). In laparoscopic surgery, a small incision is made in the patient, and a laparoscope is placed inside the body cavity. The surgeon typically views the surgical site on a two-dimensional screen rather than in three-dimensions as in open surgery (Chan et al., 1997). The two-dimensional camera image imposes perceptual and cognitive demands on the surgeon, such as impaired depth perception (Chan et al., 1997; DeLucia & Griswold, 2011) and a limited field-of-view of the site (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011). The present study examined whether top-view and side-view camera angles, which putatively impose different cognitive demands (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011), would differentially affect the resumption lag in a visual-motor task. Participants completed a peg transfer task in which they were interrupted with a mental rotation task of different durations and rotation angles. The duration of the mental rotation task was either short (6 s) or long (12 s), representing relatively low and high cognitive demands, respectively. Smaller rotation angles (0, 60, and 300 degrees from vertical) and greater rotation angles (120, 180 and 240 degrees from vertical) presumably imposed smaller and larger cognitive demands, respectively. Resumption lag was measured as the time between the end of the interruption and the first time a peg was touched in the resumed peg transfer task. Participants needed significantly more time to resume the peg transfer task with the side view compared to the top view, and with the longer mental rotation task duration compared to the shorter duration. The main effect of rotation angle was not significant. The side view also resulted in higher ratings of mental demand, effort, and frustration on the Raw Task Load Index (RTLX), the ratings-only portion of the NASA-TLX (Hart, 2006). Thus, a visual-motor task that is higher in cognitive demand can result in more time to resume a primary task following an interruption. Practical implications are that camera viewing angles associated with lower cognitive demands should be preferred in the operating room when feasible, and that interruption durations should be minimized. However, results also indicated that the side view resulted in longer movement times than the top view, even without an interruption, suggesting that factors other than cognitive demands may account for effects of camera angle on resumption lag; this should be examined in future research.


Author(s):  
Natalie R. Lodinger ◽  
Patricia R. DeLucia

Objective: To determine whether top-view and side-view camera angles, which putatively impose different cognitive demands, differentially affect the resumption lag in a visual-motor task relevant to laparoscopic surgery. Background: Prior research showed that the time to resume a primary task after performing an interrupting task (resumption lag) increases with increases in the subjective workload of the primary task. Camera views used in laparoscopic surgery provide different views of the anatomy and have different cognitive costs and associated levels of workload. Method: Participants completed a peg transfer task while interrupted with a mental rotation task of different durations and angles of stimulus rotation. Results: Participants required significantly more time to resume the peg transfer task when using a side view than a top view and when interrupted for a longer duration. Participants’ ratings of subjective workload were consistent with these patterns of performance data; the side view resulted in longer resumption lags and was rated as greater in mental demand. Additionally, the time needed to resume the peg transfer task decreased across trials for both views. Conclusion: More time is required to resume an interrupted visual-motor task when it is more cognitively demanding than when it is less cognitively demanding possibly due to needing more time to learn the visual-motor mapping of the task higher in cognitive demand. Application: Training for laparoscopic surgery should include interruptions to allow surgeons to practice resuming a surgery-related task after an interruption and consequently shorten the time needed to resume the surgery-related task.


2011 ◽  
Vol 30 (12) ◽  
pp. 2835-2839 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ling Wang ◽  
Zhao-da Zhu ◽  
Dai-yin Zhu
Keyword(s):  

2017 ◽  
Vol 898 ◽  
pp. 1312-1317 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hong Yu Ding ◽  
Pei Kun Qiu ◽  
Yuan Fei Han ◽  
Zhong Gang Sun ◽  
Jie Huang ◽  
...  

A preliminary investigation of the post heat treatment for Ti6Al4V tensile specimens fabricated via selective laser melting (SLM) are reported. The resultant microstructure and mechanical behavior at room temperature, normal and parallel to the building direction, was investigated in detail. Moreover, the as-prepared samples were compared with the samples post heat treated at 600°C~800°C for 2 h. It was found that the SLM fabricated samples consisted of long columnar original β grains together with parallel acicular α' martensitic structure dominated in the β matrix, which was observed in the side view, and fully equaixed β grains pattern in the top view. The post heat treatment did not completely disrupt the layered structure, but leading to the acicular α' martensites decomposing into α platelets distributed in a metastable β matrix. Ultimate tensile strength and elongation were analyzed and explained based on the microstructure evolution. After post heat treatment, an increase in elongation was observed for the tensile samples, inducing more ductile like morphology after fracture.


Author(s):  
Kenya Freeman ◽  
Michael S. Wogalter

Carpal tunnel syndrome (CTS) is an upper-extremity disorder that can cause chronic pain and disability. Although CTS can arise from a wide variety of repetitive tasks with awkward hand/arm positioning, a large percentage of cases are attributed to, or exacerbated by, computer input devices such as keyboards. One potential way to reduce the development of CTS in keyboard users is to warn them about the disease's early symptoms so that corrective actions might be taken before the disease becomes more severe. The present research systematically examines one of the components of a potential CTS warning, pictorial symbols. Participants examined a set of 12 ANSI Z535 style warnings with one, two or four pictorials. They then evaluated them on their perceived effectiveness, specifically on their ability to inform and motivate users to use correct arm and hand posture to avoid further CTS development. The evaluations involved estimating the percentage of people that would comply with the pictorial message if it were located on or near a keyboard. Individual pictorial symbols in a top or a side view of the hands, arms, and wrists, depicted the incorrect posture overlaid with either an “X” (cross-out) or “prohibition symbol” (circle-slash) or the correct posture with no overlay. Warnings with four pictorials (with both postures and views) were given significantly higher evaluations than warnings with one or two symbols. In the one and two pictorial conditions, the top view was preferred over the side view. The two prohibition symbols, shown together with views of the incorrect postures, were perceived to be better than the views of correct postures (with no prohibition symbol). The two prohibition symbol conditions did not differ. The results could serve as a partial basis for the development of a complete CTS warning that also includes textual information. Implications for the benefits of multi-symbol warning messages are offered.


Author(s):  
YU ZHOU ◽  
YINFEI YANG ◽  
MENG YI ◽  
XIANG BAI ◽  
WENYU LIU ◽  
...  

Indoor environment is a common scene in our everyday life, and detecting and tracking multiple targets in this environment is a key component for many applications. However, this task still remains challenging due to limited space, intrinsic target appearance variation, e.g. full or partial occlusion, large pose deformation, and scale change. In the proposed approach, we give a novel framework for detection and tracking in indoor environments, and extend it to robot navigation. One of the key components of our approach is a virtual top view created from an RGB-D camera, which is named ground plane projection (GPP). The key advantage of using GPP is the fact that the intrinsic target appearance variation and extrinsic noise is far less likely to appear in GPP than in a regular side-view image. Moreover, it is a very simple task to determine free space in GPP without any appearance learning even from a moving camera. Hence GPP is very different from the top-view image obtained from a ceiling mounted camera. We perform both object detection and tracking in GPP. Two kinds of GPP images are utilized: gray GPP, which represents the maximal height of 3D points projecting to each pixel, and binary GPP, which is obtained by thresholding the gray GPP. For detection, a simple connected component labeling is used to detect footprints of targets in binary GPP. For tracking, a novel Pixel Level Association (PLA) strategy is proposed to link the same target in consecutive frames in gray GPP. It utilizes optical flow in gray GPP, which to our best knowledge has never been done before. Then we "back project" the detected and tracked objects in GPP to original, side-view (RGB) images. Hence we are able to detect and track objects in the side-view (RGB) images. Our system is able to robustly detect and track multiple moving targets in real time. The detection process does not rely on any target model, which means we do not need any training process. Moreover, tracking does not require any manual initialization, since all entering objects are robustly detected. We also extend the novel framework to robot navigation by tracking. As our experimental results demonstrate, our approach can achieve near prefect detection and tracking results. The performance gain in comparison to state-of-the-art trackers is most significant in the presence of occlusion and background clutter.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document